[1] 高鑫, 周仁来, 董云英. (2013). 积极情绪对言语和空间工作记忆的选择性影响. 中国临床心理学杂志, 21(2), 175-179. [2] 杨仕云. (2009). 飞行员动态空间表征建构的实验研究 (博士学位论文). 陕西师范大学,西安. [3] 朱荣娟, 王梓宇, 游旭群. (2019). 经颅直流电刺激联合空间工作记忆训练对心理旋转能力的影响. 第二十二届全国心理学学术会议摘要集, 杭州. [4] Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136-R140. [5] Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1-29. [6] Balaban C. Z., Karimpur H., Röser F., & Hamburger K. (2017). Turn left where you felt unhappy: How affect influences landmark-based wayfinding. Cognitive Processing, 18(2), 135-144. [7] Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2007). Cognitive dissonance and the perception of natural environments. Psychological Science, 18(10), 917-921. [8] Brunyé T. T., Mahoney C. R., Augustyn J. S., & Taylor H. A. (2009). Emotional state and local versus global spatial memory. Acta Psychologica, 130(2), 138-146. [9] Burgess, N. (2006). Spatial memory: How egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 551-557. [10] Caragea C., Squicciarini A., Stehle S., Neppalli K., & Tapia A. (2014). Mapping moods: Geo-mapped sentiment analysis during hurricane sandy. In ISCRAM 2014 conference proceedings - 11th international conference on information systems for crisis response and management, Pennsylvania State University. [11] Chan E., Baumann O., Bellgrove M. A., & Mattingley J. B. (2012). From objects to landmarks: The function of visual location information in spatial navigation. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 304. [12] Chapman, E. H. (1962). Reviewed Work: The image of the city by Kevin Lynch. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 21(1), 91. [13] Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 393-399. [14] Denis M., Michon P. E., & Tom, A. (2007). Assisting pedestrian wayfinding in urban settings: Why references to landmarks are crucial in direction-giving In G L Allen (Ed), Applied spatial cognition: From research to cognitive technology (pp 25-51) Psychology Press Why references to landmarks are crucial in direction-giving. In G. L. Allen (Ed.), Applied spatial cognition: From research to cognitive technology (pp. 25-51). Psychology Press. [15] Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183-201. [16] Etienne, A. S. (1992). Navigation of a small mammal by dead reckoning and local cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(2), 48-52. [17] Galvez-Pol A., Nadal M., & Kilner J. M. (2021). Emotional representations of space vary as a function of peoples' affect and interoceptive sensibility. Scientific Reports, 11, Article 16150. [18] Gardony A., Brunyé T. T., Mahoney C. R., & Taylor H. A. (2011). Affective states influence spatial cue utilization during navigation. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 20(3), 223-240. [19] Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34-40. [20] Götz F. M., Stieger S., Gosling S. D., Potter J., & Rentfrow P. J. (2020). Physical topography is associated with human personality. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(11), 1135-1144. [21] Gray J. R., Braver T. S., & Raichle M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(6), 4115-4120. [22] Hauthal, E., & Burghardt, D. (2016). Mapping space-related emotions out of user-generated photo metadata considering grammatical issues. The Cartographic Journal, 53(1), 78-90. [23] Ishikawa T., Fujiwara H., Imai O., & Okabe A. (2008). Wayfinding with a GPS-based mobile navigation system: A comparison with maps and direct experience. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(1), 74-82. [24] Jackson, R. E., & Cormack, L. K. (2007). Evolved navigation theory and the descent illusion. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(3), 353-362. [25] Kontaris I., East B. S., & Wilson D. A. (2020). Behavioral and neurobiological convergence of odor, mood and emotion: A review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, Article 35. [26] Lavric A., Rippon G., & Gray J. R. (2003). Threat-evoked anxiety disrupts spatial working memory performance: An attentional account. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(5), 489-504. [27] Levine, L. J., & Edelstein, R. S. (2009). Emotion and memory narrowing: A review and goal-relevance approach. Cognition and Emotion, 23(5), 833-875. [28] Li X. Y., Li X. B., & Luo Y. J. (2005). Anxiety and attentional bias for threat: An event-related potential study. NeuroReport, 16(13), 1501-1505. [29] Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31(3), 301-306. [30] Mennis J., Mason M., & Ambrus A. (2018). Urban greenspace is associated with reduced psychological stress among adolescents: A Geographic Ecological Momentary Assessment (GEMA) analysis of activity space. Landscape and Urban Planning, 174, 1-9. [31] Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). Danger expectancies and insight in acrophobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(2), 215-221. [32] Michon, P. E., & Denis, M. (2001). When and why are visual landmarks used in giving directions? In D. R. Montello (Ed.), Spatial information theory.(pp. 292-305). Springer. [33] Palmiero M., Nori R., Rogolino C., D' Amico S., & Piccardi L. (2015). Situated navigational working memory: The role of positive mood. Cognitive Processing, 16(1), 327-330. [34] Palmiero, M., & Piccardi, L. (2017). The role of emotional landmarks on topographical memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 763. [35] Pánek, J., & Benediktsson, K. (2017). Emotional mapping and its participatory potential: Opinions about cycling conditions in Reykjavík, Iceland. Cities, 61, 65-73. [36] Piccardi L., Berthoz A., Baulac M., Denos M., Dupont S., Samson S., & Guariglia C. (2010). Different spatial memory systems are involved in small- and large-scale environments: Evidence from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Experimental Brain Research, 206(2), 171-177. [37] Piccardi L., Bianchini F., Argento O., De Nigris A., Maialetti A., Palermo L., & Guariglia C. (2013). The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): Standardization of the topographical memory test in an Italian population. Neurological Sciences, 34(6), 971-978. [38] Piccardi L., Iaria G., Ricci M., Bianchini F., Zompanti L., & Guariglia C. (2008). Walking in the Corsi test: Which type of memory do you need? Neuroscience Letters, 432(2), 127-131. [39] Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 110-122. [40] Rachman, S., & Cuk, M. (1992). Fearful distortions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(6), 583-589. [41] Raubal, M., & Winter, S. (2002). Enriching wayfinding instructions with local landmarks. In M. J. Egenhofer & D. M. Mark (Eds.), Geographic information science.(pp. 243-259). Springer. [42] Riener C. R., Stefanucci J. K., Proffitt D. R., & Clore G. (2011). An effect of mood on the perception of geographical slant. Cognition and Emotion, 25(1), 174-182. [43] Ruotolo F., Claessen M. H. G., & van der Ham, I. J. M. (2019). Putting emotions in routes: The influence of emotionally laden landmarks on spatial memory. Psychological Research, 83(5), 1083-1095. [44] Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 527-561). The Guilford Press. [45] Sinai M. J., Ooi T. L., & He Z. J. (1998). Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance. Nature, 395(6701), 497-500. [46] Stefanucci, J. K., & Storbeck, J. (2009). Don' t look down: Emotional arousal elevates height perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(1), 131-145. [47] Storbeck, J., & Maswood, R. (2016). Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory capacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cognition and Emotion, 30(5), 925-938. [48] Teachman B. A., Stefanucci J. K., Clerkin E. M., Cody M. W., & Proffitt D. R. (2008). A new mode of fear expression: Perceptual bias in height fear. Emotion, 8(2), 296-301. |