[1] 程婕婷, 张斌, 汪新建. (2015). 道德: 刻板印象内容的新维度. 心理学探新, 35(5), 442-447. [2] 代涛涛, 佐斌, 温芳芳. (2014). 社会认知中热情与能力的补偿效应. 心理科学进展, 22(3), 502-511. [3] 王妍, 罗跃嘉. (2005). 大学生面孔表情材料的标准化及其评定. 中国临床心理学杂志, 13(4), 396-398. [4] 喻丰, 彭凯平, 董蕊, 柴方圆, 韩婷婷. (2013). 道德人格研究: 范式与分歧. 心理科学进展, 21(12), 2235-2244. [5] 喻丰, 彭凯平, 韩婷婷, 柴方圆, 柏阳. (2011). 道德困境之困境——情与理的辩争. 心理科学进展, 19(11), 1702-1712. [6] Brambilla M., Carraro L., Castelli L., & Sacchi S. (2019). Changing impressions: Moral character dominates impression updating. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 82, 64-73. [7] Brambilla M., Hewstone M., & Colucci F. P. (2013). Enhancing moral virtues: Increased perceived outgroup morality as a mediator of intergroup contact effects. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 16(5), 648-657. [8] Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32(4), 397-408. [9] Brambilla M., Sacchi S., Rusconi P., Cherubini P., & Yzerbyt V. Y. (2012). You want to give a good impression? Be honest! Moral traits dominate group impression formation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 149-166. [10] Christy A. G., Kim J., Vess M., Schlegel R. J., & Hicks J. A. (2017). The reciprocal relationship between perceptions of moral goodness and knowledge of others' true selves. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(8), 910-917. [11] Cone, J., & Ferguson, M. J. (2015). He did what? The role of diagnosticity in revising implicit evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(1), 37-57. [12] Cone J., Flaharty K., & Ferguson M. J. (2019). Believability of evidence matters for correcting social impressions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(20), 9802-9807. [13] Cone J., Mann T. C., & Ferguson M. J. (2017). Changing our implicit minds: How, when, and why implicit evaluations can be rapidly revised. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 131-199. [14] Dunning, D. (2004). On the motives underlying social cognition. In M. B. Brewer & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Emotion and motivation (pp. 137-164).Blackwell. [15] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., & Lang A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160. [16] FeldmanHall, O., & Shenhav, A. (2019). Resolving uncertainty in a social world. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 426-435. [17] Ferguson M. J., Mann T. C., Cone J., & Shen X. (2019). When and how implicit first impressions can be updated. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 331-336. [18] Fiske S. T., Cuddy A. J. C., & Glick P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77-83. [19] Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Moral character in person perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 38-44. [20] Hartley A. G., Furr R. M., Helzer E. G., Jayawickreme E., Velasquez K. R., & Fleeson W. (2016). Morality' s centrality to liking, respecting, and understanding others. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(7), 648-657. [21] Heiphetz, L. (2019). Moral essentialism and generosity among children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(12), 2077-2090. [22] Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. The Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366-395. [23] Koch A., Yzerbyt V., Abele A., Ellemers N., & Fiske S. T. (2021). Social evaluation: Comparing models across interpersonal, intragroup, intergroup, several-group, and many-group contexts. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 1-68. [24] Leach C. W., Bilali R., & Pagliaro S. (2015). Groups and morality. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, J. F. Dovidio, & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 2. Group processes (pp. 123-149). American Psychological Association. [25] Luttrell A., Sacchi S., & Brambilla M. (2022). Changing impressions in competence-oriented domains: The primacy of morality endures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 98, 104246. [26] Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2015). Can we undo our first impressions? The role of reinterpretation in reversing implicit evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(6), 823-849. [27] Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2017). Reversing implicit first impressions through reinterpretation after a two-day delay. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 122-127. [28] Mende-Siedlecki P., Baron S. G., & Todorov A. (2013). Diagnostic value underlies asymmetric updating of impressions in the morality and ability domains. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(50), 19406-19415. [29] Mende-Siedlecki, P., & Todorov, A. (2016). Neural dissociations between meaningful and mere inconsistency in impression updating. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(9), 1489-1500. [30] Piazza J., Landy J. F., & Goodwin G. P. (2014). Cruel nature: Harmfulness as an important, overlooked dimension in judgments of moral standing. Cognition, 131(1), 108-124. [31] Sanbonmatsu D. M., Mazur D., Behrends A. A., & Moore S. M. (2015). The role of the baserate frequency of correspondent behavior and trait stereotypes in attribution: Building on Rothbart and Park (1986). Social Cognition, 33(4), 255-283. [32] Shen, X., & Ferguson, M. J. (2021). How resistant are implicit impressions of facial trustworthiness? When new evidence leads to durable updating. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 97, 104219. [33] Siegel J. Z., Mathys C., Rutledge R. B., & Crockett M. J. (2018). Beliefs about bad people are volatile. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(10), 750-756. [34] Strohminger N., Knobe J., & Newman G. (2017). The true self: A psychological concept distinct from the self. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(4), 551-560. [35] Trafimow, D., & Trafimow, S. (1999). Mapping perfect and imperfect duties onto hierarchically and partially restrictive trait dimensions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(6), 687-697. |