[1] 陈雪飞, 利振华, 聂衍刚. (2023). 不只是努力抑制——自我控制策略及其使用机制. 心理科学进展, 31(8), 1528-1540. [2] 谷莉, 白学军, 王芹. (2014). 奖惩对行为抑制能力和自主生理活动影响的时效性. 心理学报, 46(10), 1476-1485. [3] 李政汉, 杨国春, 南威治, 李琦, 刘勋. (2018). 冲突解决过程中认知控制的注意调节机制. 心理科学进展, 26(6), 966-974. [4] 王利刚, 谢东杰, 樊春雷, 高文斌. (2014). 自我控制的干预技术研究: 基于双系统理论. 中国临床心理学杂志, 22(2), 377-380, 372. [5] 王宴庆, 陈安涛, 胡学平, 尹首航. (2019). 奖赏通过增强信号监测提升认知控制. 心理学报, 51(1), 48-57. [6] 姚琦, 乐国安. (2009). 动机理论的新发展: 调节定向理论. 心理科学进展, 17(6), 1264-1273. [7] Crowe, E., & Tory Higgins, E. (1997). Regulatory focus and strategic inclinations: Promotion and prevention in decision-making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69(2), 117-132. [8] Dholakia U. M., Gopinath M., Bagozzi R. P., & Nataraajan R. (2006). The role of regulatory focus in the experience and self-control of desire for temptations. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16(2), 163-175. [9] Duckworth A. L., Gendler T. S., & Gross J. J. (2016). Situational strategies for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 35-55. [10] Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(3), 508-525. [11] Gillebaart, M. (2018). The 'operational' definition of self-control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1231. [12] Hedgcock W. M., Vohs K. D., & Rao A. R. (2012). Reducing self-control depletion effects through enhanced sensitivity to implementation: Evidence from fMRI and behavioral studies. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(4), 486-495. [13] Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280-1300. [14] Hofmann W., Baumeister R. F., Förster G., & Vohs K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1318-1335. [15] Hofmann W., Friese M., & Strack F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(2), 162-176. [16] Idson L. C., Liberman N., & Tory Higgins E. (2000). Distinguishing gains from nonlosses and losses from nongains: A regulatory focus perspective on hedonic intensity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36(3), 252-274. [17] Inzlicht M., Werner K. M., Briskin J. L., & Roberts B. W. (2021). Integrating models of self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 319-345. [18] Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 131-141. [19] Ledgerwood, A., & Boydstun, A. E. (2014). Sticky prospects: Loss frames are cognitively stickier than gain frames. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 376-385. [20] Leduc-Cummings I., Werner K. M., Milyavskaya M., Dominick J. K., & Cole S. (2022). Experiencing obstacles during goal pursuit: The role of goal motivation and trait self-control. Journal of Research in Personality, 99, Article 104231. [21] Legault, L., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Self-determination, self-regulation, and the brain: Autonomy improves performance by enhancing neuroaffective responsiveness to self-regulation failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(1), 123-138. [22] Lisjak M., Molden D. C., & Lee A. Y. (2012). Primed interference: The cognitive and behavioral costs of an incongruity between chronic and primed motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(5), 889-909. [23] Malaviya, P., & Brendl, C. M. (2014). Do hedonic motives moderate regulatory focus motives? Evidence from the framing of persuasive messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 1-19. [24] Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3-19. [25] Milyavskaya M., Inzlicht M., Hope N., & Koestner R. (2015). Saying "no" to temptation: Want-to motivation improves self-regulation by reducing temptation rather than by increasing self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(4), 677-693. [26] Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247-259. [27] Myrseth, K. O. R., & Fishbach, A. (2009). Self-control: A function of knowing when and how to exercise restraint. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 247-252. [28] Newheiser A. K., Barreto M., Ellemers N., Derks B., & Scheepers D. (2015). Regulatory focus moderates the social performance of individuals who conceal a stigmatized identity. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54(4), 787-797. [29] Oertig D., Schüler J., Schnelle J., Brandstätter V., Roskes M., & Elliot A. J. (2013). Avoidance goal pursuit depletes self-regulatory resources. Journal of Personality, 81(4), 365-375. [30] Ouyang Y., Yang G. C., Zhu Y., Tan Q. B., & Liu X. (2020). Better regulatory performance without greater cognitive resource expenditure: The effect of motivational states on self-regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 166, Article 110170. [31] Ouyang Y., Zhu Y., Fan W., Tan Q. B., & Zhong Y. P. (2015). People higher in self-control do not necessarily experience more happiness: Regulatory focus also affects subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 406-411. [32] Raio, C. M., & Glimcher, P. W. (2021). Quantifying the subjective cost of self-control in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(35), Article e2018726118. [33] Rodrigues D. L., Huic A., Lopes D., & Kumashiro M. (2019). Regulatory focus in relationships and conflict resolution strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 142, 116-121. [34] Roskes M., Elliot A. J., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Why is avoidance motivation problematic, and what can be done about it? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 133-138. [35] Roskes M., Elliot A. J., Nijstad B. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2013). Time pressure undermines performance more under avoidance than approach motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(6), 803-813. [36] Scarpina, F., & Tagini, S. (2017). The Stroop Color and Word Test. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(557), 1-8. [37] Shenhav A., Musslick S., Lieder F., Kool W., Griffiths T. L., Cohen J. D., Botvinick M. M. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40(1), 99-124. [38] Trawalter, S., & Richeson, J. A. (2006). Regulatory focus and executive function after interracial interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 406-412. [39] Wennerhold L., Friese M., & Vazire S. (2020). Why self-report measures of self-control and inhibition tasks do not substantially correlate. Collabra: Psychology, 6(1), Article 9. [40] Werner, K. M., & Ford, B. Q. (2023). Self-control: An integrative framework. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 17(5), Article e12738. [41] Werner, K. M., & Milyavskaya, M. (2019). Motivation and self-regulation: The role of want-to motivation in the processes underlying self-regulation and self-control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13(1), Article e12425. [42] Winterheld, H. A., & Simpson, J. A. (2011). Seeking security or growth: A regulatory focus perspective on motivations in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(5), 935-954. [43] Yoon Y., Sarial-Abi G., & Gürhan-Canli Z. (2012). Effect of regulatory focus on selective information processing. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 93-110. |