[1] 龚群虎. (2005). 双语聋教育的理论和实践. 华夏出版社.. [2] 顾笙韵, 张吉生. (2017). 手语音系研究及其理论模型. 外国语, 40(1), 52-65. [3] 李俊宏, 丁国盛. (2013). 手语和口语理解及产生的脑机制对比. 心理科学进展, 21(9), 1560-1569. [4] 祁志强, 彭聃龄. (2010). 语音加工的脑机制研究: 现状、困惑及展望. 北京师范大学学报(社会科学版), 4, 40-47. [5] 邱云峰, 姚登峰, 李荣, 刘春达. (2018). 中国手语语言学概论. 中国国际广播出版社.. [6] Andin J., Fransson P., Dahlström Ö., Rönnberg J., & Rudner M. (2019). The neural basis of arithmetic and phonology in deaf signing individuals. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(7), 813-825. [7] Andin J., Fransson P., Rönnberg J., & Rudner M. (2015). Phonology and arithmetic in the language-calculation network. Brain and Language, 143, 97-105. [8] Anthony J. L., Williams J. M., Aghara R. G., Dunkelberger M., Novak B., & Mukherjee A. D. (2010). Assessment of individual differences in phonological representation. Reading and Writing, 23(8), 969-994. [9] Banaszkiewicz A., Bola Ł., Matuszewski J., Szczepanik M., Kossowski B., Mostowski P., Rutkowski P., Sliwinska M., Jednorog K., Emmorey K., & Marchewka A. (2021). The role of the superior parietal lobule in lexical processing of sign language: Insights from fMRI and TMS. Cortex, 135, 240-254. [10] Brentari, D. (2012). Phonology. In R.Pfau, M. Steinbach, & B. Woll (Eds.), Sign language: An international handbook (pp. 21-54). De Gruyter Mouton. [11] Brentari, D. (2019). Sign language phonology. Cambridge University Press.. [12] Cardin V., Orfanidou E., Kästner L., Rönnberg J., Woll B., Capek C. M., & Rudner M. (2016). Monitoring different phonological parameters of sign language engages the same cortical language network but distinctive perceptual ones. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(1), 20-40. [13] Cardin V., Orfanidou E., Rönnberg J., Capek C. M., Rudner M., & Woll B. (2013). Dissociating cognitive and sensory neural plasticity in human superior temporal cortex. Nature Communications, 4(1), Article 1473. [14] Carreiras M., Gutiérrez-Sigut E., Baquero S., & Corina D., (2008). Lexical processing in Spanish Sign Language (LSE). Journal of Memory and Language, 58(1), 100-122. [15] Caselli N. K., Emmorey K., & Cohen-Goldberg A. M. (2021). The signed mental lexicon: Effects of phonological neighborhood density, iconicity, and childhood language experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 121, Article 104282. [16] Dye, M. W. G., & Shih, S. I. (2006). Phonological priming in British Sign Language. In L. M. Goldstein, D. H. Whalen, & C. T. Best (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 8 (pp. 241-264). Mouton de Gruyter. [17] Emmorey K., Xu J., & Braun A. (2011). Neural responses to meaningless pseudosigns: Evidence for sign-based phonetic processing in superior temporal cortex. Brain and Language, 117(1), 34-38. [18] Fenlon J., Cormier K., & Brentari D. (2018). The phonology of sign languages. In S. J. Hannahs & A. R. K. Bosch (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of phonological theory (pp. 453-475). Routledge. [19] Grushkin, D. A. (2017). Writing signed languages: What for? What form? American Annals of the Deaf, 161(5), 509-527. [20] Gutiérrez E., Müller O., Baus C., & Carreiras M. (2012). Electrophysiological evidence for phonological priming in Spanish Sign Language lexical access. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1335-1346. [21] Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402. [22] Hu Z. G., Wang W. J., Liu H. Y., Peng D. L., Yang Y. H., Li K. C., & Ding G. S. (2011). Brain activations associated with sign production using word and picture inputs in deaf signers. Brain and Language, 116(2), 64-70. [23] Liddell, S. K., & Johnson, R. E. (1989). American Sign Language: The phonological base. Sign Language Studies, 64(1), 195-278. [24] Lieberman, A. M., & Borovsky, A. (2020). Lexical recognition in deaf children learning American Sign Language: Activation of semantic and phonological features of signs. Language Learning, 70(4), 935-973. [25] Liu L. F., Yan X., Liu J., Xia M. R., Lu C. M., Emmorey K., Chu M., & Ding G. S. (2017). Graph theoretical analysis of functional network for comprehension of sign language. Brain Research, 1671, 55-66. [26] MacSweeney M., Capek C. M., Campbell R., & Woll B. (2008). The signing brain: The neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 432-440. [27] MacSweeney, M., & Emmorey, K. (2020). The neurobiology of sign language processing. In D. Poeppel, G. R. Mangun, & M. S. Gazzaniga (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 849-857). The MIT Press. [28] MacSweeney M., Waters D., Brammer M. J., Woll B., & Goswami U. (2008). Phonological processing in deaf signers and the impact of age of first language acquisition. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1369-1379. [29] Meade G., Lee B., Massa N., Holcomb P. J., Midgley K. E., & Emmorey K. (2021). The organization of the American Sign Language lexicon: Comparing one- and two-parameter ERP phonological priming effects across tasks. Brain and Language, 218, Article 104960. [30] Newman A. J., Supalla T., Fernandez N., Newport E. L., & Bavelier D. (2015). Neural systems supporting linguistic structure, linguistic experience, and symbolic communication in sign language and gesture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(37), 11684-11689. [31] Ortega, G. (2017). Iconicity and sign lexical acquisition: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1280. [32] Perlman M., Little H., Thompson B., & Thompson R. L. (2018). Iconicity in signed and spoken vocabulary: A comparison between American Sign Language, British Sign Language, English, and Spanish. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1433. [33] Petitto L. A., Zatorre R. J., Gauna K., Nikelske E. J., Dostie D., & Evans A. C. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: Implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(25), 13961-13966. [34] Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. NeuroImage, 62(2), 816-847. [35] Rudner M., Karlsson T., Gunnarsson J., & Rönnberg J. (2013). Levels of processing and language modality specificity in working memory. Neuropsychologia, 51(4), 656-666. [36] Rudner M., Orfanidou E., Kästner L., Cardin V., Woll B., Capek C., & Rönnberg J. (2019). Neural networks supporting phoneme monitoring are modulated by phonology but not lexicality or iconicity: Evidence from British and Swedish Sign Language. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, Article 374. [37] Sandler, W. (2017). The challenge of sign language phonology. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 43-63. [38] Thierfelder P., Wigglesworth G., & Tang G. (2020). Sign phonological parameters modulate parafoveal preview effects in deaf readers. Cognition, 201, Article 104286. [39] Thompson R. L., Vinson D. P., Fox N., & Vigliocco G. (2013). Is lexical access driven by temporal order or perceptual salience? Evidence from British Sign Language. Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the cognitive science society, Berlin, Germany. [40] van der Kooij, E. (2002). Phonological categories in Sign Language of the Netherlands: The role of phonetic implementation and iconicity (Unpublished Doctorial dissertation). Leiden University. [41] Villameriel S., Costello B., Dias P., Giezen M., & Carreiras M. (2019). Language modality shapes the dynamics of word and sign recognition. Cognition, 191, Article 103979. [42] Wienholz A., Nuhbalaoglu D., Steinbach M., Herrmann A., & Mani N. (2021). Phonological priming in German Sign Language: An eye tracking study using the Visual World Paradigm. Sign Language & Linguistics, 24(1), 4-35. [43] Winsler K., Midgley K. J., Grainger J., & Holcomb P. J. (2018). An electrophysiological megastudy of spoken word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(8), 1063-1082. |