[1] 陈勃, 杨瑞娟, 邓稳根. (2013). 观察者公正敏感性对不公正信息加工的影响. 心理学探新, 33(6), 507-512. [2] 陈庆, 何泉, 陈广耀, 郭悦智, 张荷婧, 何先友. (2017). 复杂情境下不同角度及思维方式的决策表现差异: 决策视角-心理距离的作用. 心理学报, 49(3), 383-392. [3] 高娟, 王鹏, 王晓田, 孙倩, 刘永芳. (2020). 得失情境下他人参照点及心理距离对自我-他人利益权衡的影响. 心理学报, 52(5), 633-644. [4] 李明晖, 饶俪琳. (2017). 解释水平视角下的道德判断. 心理科学进展, 25(8), 1423-1430. [5] 张振, 齐春辉, 付淑英, 王青春, 熊建萍. (2020). 社会距离和性别对公平关注的影响. 心理与行为研究, 18(1), 100-106. [6] 周晓林, 胡捷, 彭璐. (2015). 社会情境影响公平感知及相关行为的神经机制. 心理与行为研究, 13(5), 591-598. [7] Aguilar P., Brussino S., & Fernández-Dols J. M. (2013). Psychological distance increases uncompromising consequentialism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 449-452. [8] Alper, S. (2020). Explaining the complex effect of construal level on moral and political attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(2), 115-120. [9] Bar-Anan Y., Liberman N., & Trope Y. (2006). The association between psychological distance and construal level: Evidence from an implicit association test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(4), 609-622. [10] Borgida, E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1977). The differential impact of abstract vs. concrete information on decisions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 7(3), 258-271. [11] Carroll, J. S. (1978). The effect of imagining an event on expectations for the event: An interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(1), 88-96. [12] Carter A. B., Bobocel D. R., & Brockner J. (2020). When to explain why or how it happened: Tailoring accounts to fit observers' construal level. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 26(1), 158-170. [13] Ding X. F., Feng N., He T. Y., Cheng X. R., & Fan Z. (2020). Can mental time lines co-exist in 3D space? Acta Psychologica, 207, Article 103084. [14] Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223-241. [15] Eyal, T., & Liberman, N. (2012). Morality and psychological distance: A construal level theory perspective. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The social psychology of morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil (pp. 185-202). American Psychological Association. [16] Eyal T., Liberman N., & Trope Y. (2008). Judging near and distant virtue and vice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1204-1209. [17] Fennell P. B., Coleman J. T., & Kuo A. (2020). The moderating role of donation quantifiers on price fairness judgments. Journal of Business Research, 110, 464-473. [18] Fetchenhauer, D., & Huang, X. (2004). Justice sensitivity and distributive decisions in experimental games. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(5), 1015-1029. [19] Freitas A. L., Gollwitzer P., & Trope Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others' self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 739-752. [20] Fujita K., Henderson M. D., Eng J., Trope Y., & Liberman N. (2006). Spatial distance and mental construal of social events. Psychological Science, 17(4), 278-282. [21] Gamliel E., Kreiner H., & McElroy T. (2017). The effect of construal level on unethical behavior. The Journal of Social Psychology, 157(2), 211-222. [22] Gong, H., & Medin, D. L. (2012). Construal levels and moral judgment: Some complications. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(5), 628-638. [23] Gregory W. L., Cialdini R. B., & Carpenter K. M. (1982). Self-relevant scenarios as mediators of likelihood estimates and compliance: Does imagining make it so. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(1), 89-99. [24] Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998-1002. [25] Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2010). Truth from language and truth from fit: The impact of linguistic concreteness and level of construal on subjective truth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(11), 1576-1588. [26] Hofmann W., Brandt M. J., Wisneski D. C., Rockenbach B., & Skitka L. J. (2018). Moral punishment in everyday life. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(12), 1697-1711. [27] Kim H., Schnall S., Yi D. J., & White M. P. (2013). Social distance decreases responders' sensitivity to fairness in the ultimatum game. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(5), 632-638. [28] Moran T., Bornstein O., & Eyal T. (2021). The level of construal involved in the elicitation of core versus moral disgust. Emotion, 21(2), 391-404. [29] Paramita W., Septianto F., Winahjoe S., Purwanto B. M., & Candra I. D. (2020). Sharing is (not) caring? The interactive effects of power and psychological distance on tolerance of unethical behavior. Australasian Marketing Journal, 28(3), 42-49. [30] Schmitt, M. (1996). Individual differences in sensitivity to befallen injustice (SBI). Personality and Individual Differences, 21(1), 3-20. [31] Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 558-568. [32] Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403-421. [33] Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440-463. [34] Vallesi A., Binns M. A., & Shallice T. (2008). An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition, 107(2), 501-527. [35] van den Bos, K. (2003). On the subjective quality of social justice: The role of affect as information in the psychology of justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 482-498. [36] Weidman A. C., Sowden W. J., Berg M. K., & Kross E. (2020). Punish or protect? How close relationships shape responses to moral violations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(5), 693-708. [37] Wenzel, M., & Coughlin, A. M. (2020). Toward the bigger picture: Concrete and abstract thinking about a transgression, and the role of time in interpersonal forgiveness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(4), 783-798. [38] Williams L., Stein R., & Galguera-Garcia L. (2012). Beyond construal: Specifying the distinct emotional consequences of psychological distance and abstract construal. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2120157 [39] Wu Y., Leliveld M. C., & Zhou X. L. (2011). Social distance modulates recipient' s fairness consideration in the dictator game: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 88(2-3), 253-262. [40] Yu R. J., Hu P., & Zhang P. (2015). Social distance and anonymity modulate fairness consideration: An ERP study. Scientific Reports, 5(1), Article 13452. [41] Žeželj, I. L., & Jokić, B. R. (2014). Replication of experiments evaluating impact of psychological distance on moral judgment: (Eyal, Liberman & Trope, 2008; Gong & Medin, 2012). Social Psychology, 45(3), 223-231. |