[1] 常欣, 白鹤, 王沛. (2017). 双语者语言切换代价的影响因素. 心理科学进展, 25(9), 1469-1478. [2] 常欣, 白鹤, 王沛. (2018). 抑制控制能力及其训练对低熟练汉英双语者语言切换代价的影响. 外语教学与研究, 50(4), 569-583. [3] 程凯文, 邓颜蕙, 颜红梅. (2019). 第二语言学习与脑可塑性. 心理科学进展, 27(2), 209-220. [4] 焦鲁, 王瑞明, 刘聪, 沈曼琼. (2016). 语言转换影响双语认知优势的发展进程. 心理与行为研究, 14(3), 298-304, 366. [5] 李莹,赵鸿瑜,张木军,范子璇,王悦. (2023). 执行控制的双语优势效应及其调节变量:来自元分析的证据. 心理科学进展, 31(6), 970-987. [6] 刘欢欢, 陈宝国. (2015). 语言转换的认知及其神经机制. 心理科学, 38(1), 98-103. [7] 祁志强, 彭聃龄, 丁国盛. (2010). 不可预期条件下汉英双语者的语言切换研究. 心理科学, 33(5), 1051-1053. [8] 舒华, 程元善, 张厚粲. (1989). 235个图形的命名一致性、熟悉性、表象一致性和视觉复杂性评定. 心理学报, 21(4), 389-396. [9] 王敬欣, 贾丽萍, 何立媛, 张阔. (2014). 汉-英双语者图片命名任务中的语码切换代价. 心理学探新, 34(2), 147-151. [10] 王瑞明, 焦鲁, 范小月, 刘聪. (2016). 第二语言学习的利与弊: 语言与认知的角度. 华东师范大学学报(教育科学版), 34(1), 90-97. [11] 王梓宇, 孔子叶, 朱荣娟, 游旭群. (2019). 任务转换训练和执行功能可塑性. 心理科学进展, 27(10), 1667-1676. [12] 伍丽梅, 伍国华, 陈卓铭. (2017). 双语者言语产生中语码切换代价——从孤立词汇到句子语境. 心理科学进展, 25(1), 37-48. [13] Adi-Japha E., Berberich-Artzi J., & Libnawi A. (2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81(5), 1356-1366. [14] Antón E., Carreiras M., & Duñabeitia J. A. (2019). The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLoS ONE, 14(2), Article e0206770. [15] Artuso, C., & Palladino, P. (2019). The role of memory updating in shallow native and opaque second language learning. Second Language Research, 35(3), 377-396. [16] Blom E., Küntay A. C., Messer M., Verhagen J., & Leseman P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105-119. [17] Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 568-585. [18] Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491-511. [19] De Baene W., Duyck W., Brass M., & Carreiras M. (2015). Brain circuit for cognitive control is shared by task and language switching. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(9), 1752-1765. [20] Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186-204. [21] Jiao L., Liu C., Wang R. M., & Chen B. G. (2019). Working memory demand of a task modulates bilingual advantage in executive functions. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(1), 102-117. [22] Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of taskswitching training. Developmental Science, 12(6), 978-990. [23] Karlsson J., Jolles D., Koornneef A., van den Broek P., & van Leijenhorst L. (2019). Individual differences in children's comprehension of temporal relations: Dissociable contributions of working memory capacity and working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 185, 1-18. [24] Liu C., Jiao L., Wang Z. Y., Wang M. X., Wang R. M., & Wu Y. J. (2019). Symmetries of bilingual language switch costs in conflicting versus non-conflicting contexts. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(3), 624-636. [25] Liu H. H., Liang L. J., Dunlap S., Fan N., & Chen B. G. (2016). The effect of domain-general inhibition-related training on language switching: An ERP study. Cognition, 146, 264-276. [26] Magezi D. A., Khateb A., Mouthon M., Spierer L., & Annoni J. M. (2012). Cognitive control of language production in bilinguals involves a partly independent process within the domain-general cognitive control network: Evidence from task-switching and electrical brain activity. Brain and Language, 122(1), 55-63. [27] Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 25-40. [28] Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review, 35, 113-124. [29] Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535-551. [30] Seçer, I. (2016). Skills of cognitive flexibility in monolingual and bilingual younger adults. The Journal of General Psychology, 143(3), 172-184. [31] Thomas, M. S. C., & Allport, A. (2000). Language switching costs in bilingual visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(1), 44-66. [32] Timmer K., Calabria M., & Costa A. (2019). Non-linguistic effects of language switching training. Cognition, 182, 14-24. [33] Wen, H., & Dong, Y. P. (2019). How does interpreting experience enhance working memory and short-term memory: A meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 31(8), 769-784. [34] Zhang H. Y., Kang C. Y., Wu Y. J., Ma F. Y., & Guo T. M. (2015). Improving proactive control with training on language switching in bilinguals. NeuroReport, 26(6), 354-359. |