心理科学 ›› 2024, Vol. 47 ›› Issue (2): 258-266.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20240201
• 基础、实验与工效 • 下一篇
司双庆, 周思宏, 袁加锦, 杨倩**
出版日期:
2024-03-20
发布日期:
2024-02-29
通讯作者:
**杨倩,E-mail: qianyang_psy@163.com
基金资助:
Si Shuangqing, Zhou Sihong, Yuan Jiajin, Yang Qian
Online:
2024-03-20
Published:
2024-02-29
摘要: 根据控制的预期价值(expected value of control, EVC)模型,回报与代价通过调节动机共同影响认知控制的实施与分配(即回报-代价权衡)。在EVC模型外,还有许多探究动机相关因素如何影响认知控制的研究。文章首先总结梳理了可被视为回报或代价的因素及相关的实证证据;其次,基于回报-代价权衡简要讨论二者如何共同影响认知控制;最后提出未来研究可以从客观(优化权衡的计算模型)与主观(个体差异)两个方面更全面地解析回报-代价的权衡过程,以实现对认知控制的有效调控与精准干预。
司双庆, 周思宏, 袁加锦, 杨倩. 认知控制过程的影响因素:回报与代价*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(2): 258-266.
Si Shuangqing, Zhou Sihong, Yuan Jiajin, Yang Qian. Influencing Factors in the Allocation of Cognitive Control: Rewards and Costs[J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(2): 258-266.
[1] 曹思琪, 汤晨晨, 伍海燕, 刘勋. (2022). 价值计算决定何时与如何努力. 心理科学进展, 30(4), 877-887. [2] 杨倩. (2022). 负性情绪在冲突适应中的作用机制: 分离与整合视角. 心理科学进展, 30(8), 1844-1855. [3] 易伟, 梅淑婷, 郑亚. (2019). 努力: 成本还是奖赏? 心理科学进展, 27(8), 1439-1450. [4] 张孟可, 李晴, 尹首航, 陈安涛. (2021). 冲突水平的变化诱发冲突适应. 心理学报, 53(2), 128-138. [5] Aarts E., Wallace D. L., Dang L. C., Jagust W. J., Cools R., & D’Esposito M. (2014). Dopamine and the cognitive downside of a promised bonus. Psychological Science, 25(4), 1003-1009. [6] André N., Audiffren M., & Baumeister R. F. (2019). An integrative model of effortful control. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13, Article 79. [7] Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610-615. [8] Bahlmann J., Aarts E., & D’Esposito M. (2015). Influence of motivation on control hierarchy in the human frontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 3207-3217. [9] Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration: Animals spend much of their time seeking stimuli whose significance raises problems for psychology. Science, 153(3731), 25-33. [10] Boehler C. N., Hopf J. M., Stoppel C. M., & Krebs R. M. (2012). Motivating inhibition - reward prospect speeds up response cancellation. Cognition, 125(3), 498-503. [11] Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 83-113. [12] Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 356-366. [13] Botvinick M. M., Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Carter C. S., & Cohen J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652. [14] Bustamante L., Lieder F., Musslick S., Shenhav A., & Cohen J. (2021). Learning to overexert cognitive control in a Stroop task. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 21(3), 453-471. [15] Bustamante, L. A. (2022). Quantifying individual differences in the cost of cognitive and physical effort in humans (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Princeton University. [16] Bialaszek, W., & Winkielman, P. (2023). Modeling the Effort Paradox: Effort can have positive and negative impact on outcome value within an individual, across individuals, and across choice contexts (preprint) [17] Cacioppo J. T., Petty R. E., Feinstein J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197-253. [18] Camerer, C. F., & Hogarth, R. M. (1999). The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19(1), 7-42. [19] Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319-333. [20] Charpentier C. J., Bromberg-Martin E. S., & Sharot T. (2018). Valuation of knowledge and ignorance in mesolimbic reward circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(31), E7255-E7264. [21] Chew B., Blain B., Dolan R. J., & Rutledge R. B. (2021). A neurocomputational model for intrinsic reward. The Journal of Neuroscience, 41(43), 8963-8971. [22] Chiew, K. S. (2021). Revisiting positive affect and reward influences on cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 27-33. [23] Cromwell H. C., Abe N., Barrett K. C., Caldwell-Harris C., Gendolla G. H. E., Koncz R., & Sachdev P. S. (2020). Mapping the interconnected neural systems underlying motivation and emotion: A key step toward understanding the human affectome. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 113, 204-226. [24] Cubillo A., Makwana A. B., & Hare T. A. (2019). Differential modulation of cognitive control networks by monetary reward and punishment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(3), 305-317. [25] Davis C., Levitan R. D., Kaplan A. S., Carter J., Reid C., Curtis C., & Kennedy J. L. (2008). Reward sensitivity and the D2 dopamine receptor gene: A case-control study of binge eating disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 32(3), 620-628. [26] Devine S., Vassena E., & Otto A. R. (2023). More than a feeling: Physiological measures of affect index the integration of effort costs and rewards during anticipatory effort evaluation. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 23(4), 1129-1140. [27] Dignath D., Eder A. B., Steinhauser M., & Kiesel A. (2020). Conflict monitoring and the affective-signaling hypothesis—An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27(2), 193-216. [28] Dixon, M. L., & Christoff, K. (2012). The decision to engage cognitive control is driven by expected reward-value: Neural and behavioral evidence. PLoS ONE, 7(12), Article e51637. [29] Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition, 78(2), 94-98. [30] Engelmann J. B., Damaraju E., Padmala S., & Pessoa L. (2009). Combined effects of attention and motivation on visual task performance: Transient and sustained motivational effects. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, Article 4. [31] Fleischhauer M., Enge S., Brocke B., Ullrich J., Strobel A., & Strobel A. (2010). Same or different? Clarifying the relationship of need for cognition to personality and intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(1), 82-96. [32] Fröber, K., & Dreisbach, G. (2016). How sequential changes in reward magnitude modulate cognitive flexibility: Evidence from voluntary task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(2), 285-295. [33] Froböse M. I., Westbrook A., Bloemendaal M., Aarts E., & Cools R. (2020). Catecholaminergic modulation of the cost of cognitive control in healthy older adults. PLoS ONE, 15(2), Article e0229294. [34] Grahek I., Leng X., Musslick S., & Shenhav A. (2023). The cost of adjusting cognitive control: A dynamical systems approach. Paper presented at the 2023 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience. [35] Grahek I., Musslick S., & Shenhav A. (2020). A computational perspective on the roles of affect in cognitive control. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 151, 25-34. [36] Hassall C. D., Hunt L. T., & Holroyd C. B. (2022). Task-level value affects trial-level reward processing. NeuroImage, 260, Article 119456. [37] Huskey R., Craighead B., Miller M. B., & Weber R. (2018). Does intrinsic reward motivate cognitive control? A naturalistic-fMRI study based on the synchronization theory of flow. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(5), 902-924. [38] Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450-463. [39] Inzlicht M., Shenhav A., & Olivola C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337-349. [40] Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2018). Mental labour. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(12), 899-908. [41] Kool W., McGuire J. T., Rosen Z. B., & Botvinick M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 665-682. [42] Kool W., Shenhav A., & Botvinick M. M. (2017). Cognitive control as cost-benefit decision making. In T. Egner (Ed.), The Wiley handbook of cognitive control (pp. 167-189). Wiley Blackwell. [43] Kruglanski A. W., Fishbach A., Woolley K., Bélanger J. J., Chernikova M., Molinario E., & Pierro A. (2018). A structural model of intrinsic motivation: On the psychology of means-ends fusion. Psychological Review, 125(2), 165-182. [44] Kurzban, R. (2016). The sense of effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 67-70. [45] Kurzban R., Duckworth A., Kable J. W., & Myers J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661-679. [46] Leng X. M., Yee D., Ritz H., & Shenhav A. (2021). Dissociable influences of reward and punishment on adaptive cognitive control. PLoS Computational Biology, 17(12), Article e1009737. [47] Li Q., Hu N., Li Y. L., Long Q. S., Gu Y., Tang Y. C., & Chen A. T. (2021). Error-induced adaptability: Behavioral and neural dynamics of response-stimulus interval modulations on posterror slowing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(5), 851-863. [48] Lieder F., Shenhav A., Musslick S., & Griffiths T. L. (2018). Rational metareasoning and the plasticity of cognitive control. PLoS Computational Biology, 14(4), Article e1006043. [49] Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 99-112. [50] Marcowski P., Winkielman P., & Białaszek W. (2023). Effort can have positive and negative impact on outcome value within an individual, across individuals, and across choice contexts. PsyArXiv (preprint). [51] Masís J. A., Musslick S., & Cohen J. D. (2021). The value of learning and cognitive control allocation. Paper presented at the 43rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society: Animal Minds, Virtual, Online, Austria. [52] Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167-202. [53] Morris L. S., Grehl M. M., Rutter S. B., Mehta M., & Westwater M. L. (2022). On what motivates us: A detailed review of intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivation. Psychological Medicine, 52(10), 1801-1816. [54] Notebaert, W., & Braem, S. (2015). Parsing the effects of reward on cognitive control. In T. S. Braver (Ed.), Motivation and cognitive control (pp. 105-122). Routledge. [55] Otto A. R., Braem S., Silvetti M., & Vassena E. (2022). Is the juice worth the squeeze? Learning the marginal value of mental effort over time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(10), 2324-2341. [56] Otto, A. R., & Vassena, E. (2021). It' s all relative: Reward-induced cognitive control modulation depends on context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(2), 306-313. [57] Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2011). Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3419-3432. [58] Prével A., Hoofs V., & Krebs R. M. (2021). Effect of non-instructed instrumental contingency of monetary reward and positive affect in a cognitive control task. Royal Society Open Science, 8(8), Article 202002. [59] Sandra, D. A., & Otto, A. R. (2018). Cognitive capacity limitations and Need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure. Cognition, 172, 101-106. [60] Saunders B., Lin H., Milyavskaya M., & Inzlicht M. (2017). The emotive nature of conflict monitoring in the medial prefrontal cortex. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 119, 31-40. [61] Sayalı, C., & Badre, D. (2019). Neural systems of cognitive demand avoidance. Neuropsychologia, 123, 41-54. [62] Sayalı, C., & Badre, D. (2021). Neural systems underlying the learning of cognitive effort costs. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 21(4), 698-716. [63] Schouppe N., Braem S., De Houwer J., Silvetti M., Verguts T., Ridderinkhof K. R., & Notebaert W. (2015). No pain, no gain: The affective valence of congruency conditions changes following a successful response. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(1), 251-261. [64] Schouppe N., Demanet J., Boehler C. N., Ridderinkhof K. R., & Notebaert W. (2014). The role of the striatum in effort-based decision-making in the absence of reward. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(6), 2148-2154. [65] Sharp P. B., Do K. T., Lindquist K. A., Prinstein M. J., & Telzer E. H. (2022). Cognitive control deployment is flexibly modulated by social value in early adolescence. Developmental Science, 25(1), Article e13140. [66] Shenhav A., Botvinick M. M., & Cohen J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217-240. [67] Shenhav A., Musslick S., Lieder F., Kool W., Griffiths T. L., Cohen J. D., & Botvinick M. M. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40, 99-124. [68] Stürmer, B. (2011). Reward and punishment effects on error processing and conflict control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 335. [69] van Steenbergen H., Band G. P. H., & Hommel B. (2012). Reward valence modulates conflict-driven attentional adaptation: Electrophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology, 90(3), 234-241. [70] Vermeylen L., Braem S., Notebaert W., & Ruitenberg, M. F. L. (2022). The subjective evaluation of task switch cues is related to voluntary task switching. Cognition, 224, Article 105063. [71] Wang, K. S., & Delgado, M. R. (2019). Corticostriatal circuits encode the subjective value of perceived control. Cerebral Cortex, 29(12), 5049-5060. [72] Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(2), 395-415. [73] Westbrook A., Frank M. J., & Cools R. (2021). A mosaic of cost-benefit control over cortico-striatal circuitry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(8), 710-721. [74] Westbrook A., Kester D., & Braver T. S. (2013). What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PLoS ONE, 8(7), Article e68210. [75] Westbrook A., Van Den Bosch R., Määttä J. I., Hofmans L., Papadopetraki D., Cools R., & Frank M. J. (2020). Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work. Science, 367(6484), 1362-1366. [76] Yee D. M., Krug M. K., Allen A. Z., & Braver T. S. (2016). Humans integrate monetary and liquid incentives to motivate cognitive task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 2037. [77] Zhang M., Palmer C. V., Pratt S. R., McNeil M. R., & Siegle G. J. (2022). Need for cognition is associated with the interaction of reward and task-load on effort: A verification and extension study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 180, 60-67. |
[1] | 李莹, 李琳, 潘家冰, 卢笑笑, 王悦. 执行控制训练对不同工作记忆刷新能力汉英双语者语言控制的影响*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(5): 1088-1095. |
[2] | 王志静, 李富洪. 认知控制的瞳孔反应及脑机制*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(1): 2-10. |
[3] | 王冠华, 鲁嘉晨, 李红, 雷怡. 诱发高自我效能感改善自我损耗后的认知控制 *[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(6): 1282-1289. |
[4] | 于腾旭, 胡金生. 青少年适应与不适应冒险理论述评*[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(6): 1440-1446. |
[5] | 张环, 左天然, 刘一贝, 刘希平, 杨海波. 社会分享型提取诱发遗忘发生的认知机制:抑制还是非抑制?*[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(4): 802-808. |
[6] | 于 睿, 陶 云 朱晓媛 田 涛. 双语者有更好的冲突监控水平吗?[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(3): 522-529. |
[7] | 付美玲 李富洪 谢流芳. 任务切换理论争议新证据[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(5): 1085-1091. |
[8] | 陈瀛 吴瑕 汪新建. 认知控制子成分与客观风险对信任行为的影响[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(1): 187-194. |
[9] | 谢倩 张春雨 陈谢平. 付出回报失衡与高校青年教师的焦虑:心理契约破裂的中介作用和过度承诺的调节作用[J]. 心理科学, 2021, 44(4): 889-895. |
[10] | 宋婷 丁刚强 曹碧华 杜玮玮 李富洪. 负荷越大,转换更难-来自数字大小交替比较的证据[J]. 心理科学, 2021, 44(2): 290-295. |
[11] | 谢流芳 李富洪. 任务切换中的反应重复效应[J]. , 2020, 43(6): 1319-1326. |
[12] | 刘旭 岳鹏飞 白学军. 提取诱发遗忘中的相关代价与效益问题:反应抑制能力与项目竞争强度的影响[J]. , 2019, 42(5): 1039-1046. |
[13] | 谢东杰 苏彦捷. 第三方惩罚的演化与认知机制[J]. 心理科学, 2019, 42(1): 216-222. |
[14] | 李馨* 王雯 梁菲菲 马杰 杨宇 连坤予 张明哲 白学军. 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童快速命名缺陷的眼动研究[J]. 心理科学, 2019, 42(1): 43-49. |
[15] | 麦春燕 何先友. 非平衡双语者Stroop效应的时间进程[J]. 心理科学, 2017, 40(5): 1040-1046. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||