Cheesman , J . , Merikle , P. M.( 1984). Priming with and without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 36 (4) :387 - 395 Cohen Kadosh, R., Henik, A., Rubinsten, O., Mohr, H., Dori, H., Van de Ven, V., et al. (2005). Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1238–1248. Dagenbach, D., Carr, T., & Wilhelmsen, A. (1989). Task-induced strategies and near-threshold priming: Conscious influences on unconscious perception. Journal of Memory & Language, 28, 412-443 Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122 (3), 371–396. Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Cohen, L. (1998). Abstract representations of numbers in the human and animal brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 355–361. Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626–641. Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P. F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395 (6702), 597-600. Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970–974. Fias, W., & Fischer, M. H. (2005). Spatial representation of number. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 43–54). Psychology Press. Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74. Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (2000). Non-verbal numerical cognition: From reals to integers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 59–65. Gevers, W. Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006) Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica 122, 221–233 Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 32, 32–44. Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 435–448. Ishihara M, Keller PE, Rossetti Y, and Prinz W.( 2008). Horizontal spatial representations of time: evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 44: 454–461. Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 32, 662–673. Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238–300. Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520. Naccache L, Dehaene S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli.Cognition, 80(3), 215-229. Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416–442. Rusconi E, Kwan B, Giordano BL, Umilta C, and Butterworth B.( 2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99: 113–129. Santens, S., & Gevers, W., The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line, Cognition (2008), doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.01.002 Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 651–664. 郭秀艳. 实验心理学.(2004). 北京: 人民教育出版社. 宴倩,熊哲宏(2006). 先天模块与后天文化资源的相互作用论——B.Butterworth 的“数字模块”理论述评. 心理科学,29 (5) :1269 - 1271 |