[1] 黄希庭. (1993). 时距信息加工的认知研究. 西南师范大学学报(自然科学版), 18(2), 207-215. [2] 潘运, 白学军, 沈德立. (2010). 内源性注意和外源性注意条件下SOA变化对数字距离效应的影响. 心理科学, 33(3), 521-525. [3] 尹天子, 戚江丹, 刘潇. (2019). 知觉组织对时序知觉的影响——三项反应任务的证据. 心理研究, 12(4), 316-323. [4] 尹天子, 吴倩, 戚江丹. (2021). 知觉组织影响时序知觉的认知机制: ERP研究的证据. 心理学探新, 41(2), 131-135. [5] 张锋, 黄希庭, 郭秀艳. (2009). 时序知觉重复启动效应的作用机制. 心理学报, 41(3), 233-241. [6] Baruch O., Yeshurun Y., & Shore D. I. (2013). Space and time: An impact of spatial separation, apparent motion, and perceptual grouping on TOJ performance. Perception, 42(5), 551-561. [7] Buschman, T. J., & Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315(5820), 1860-1862. [8] Conci M., Böbel E., Matthias E., Keller I., Müller H. J., & Finke K. (2009). Preattentive surface and contour grouping in Kanizsa figures: Evidence from parietal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 47(3), 726-732. [9] Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-215. [10] Doallo S., Lorenzo-López L., Vizoso C., Holguín S. R., Amenedo E., Bará S., & Cadaveira F. (2004). The time course of the effects of central and peripheral cues on visual processing: An event-related potentials study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(1), 199-210. [11] Giordano A. M., McElree B., & Carrasco M. (2009). On the automaticity and flexibility of covert attention: A speed-accuracy trade-off analysis. Journal of Vision, 9(3), Article 30. [12] Jaśkowski, P. (1993). Selective attention and temporal-order judgment. Perception, 22(6), 681-689. [13] Marini, F., & Marzi, C. A. (2016). Gestalt perceptual organization of visual stimuli captures attention automatically: Electrophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, Article 446. [14] McDonald J. J., Teder-Sälejärvi W. A., Di Russo F., & Hillyard S. A. (2005). Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 1197-1202. [15] Montoro P. R., Luna D., Albert J., Santaniello G., López-Martín S., Pozo M. A., & Hinojosa J. A. (2015). A temporo-spatial analysis of the neural correlates of extrinsic perceptual grouping in vision. Neuropsychologia, 69, 118-129. [16] Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 315-330. [17] Nicol, J. R., & Shore, D. I. (2007). Perceptual grouping impairs temporal resolution. Experimental Brain Research, 183(2), 141-148. [18] Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59-63. [19] Poscoliero T., Marzi C. A., & Girelli M. (2013). Unconscious priming by illusory figures: The role of the salient region. Journal of Vision, 13(5), Article 27. [20] Radeau, M. (1994). Auditory-visual spatial interaction and modularity. Current Psychology of Cognition, 13(1), 3-51. [21] Rappaport S. J., Riddoch M. J., & Humphreys G. W. (2011). The grouping benefit in extinction: Overcoming the temporal order bias. Neuropsychologia, 49(1), 151-155. [22] Remington R. W., Johnston J. C., & Yantis S. (1992). Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 51(3), 279-290. [23] Schettino A., Rossi V., Pourtois G., & Müller M. M. (2016). Involuntary attentional orienting in the absence of awareness speeds up early sensory processing. Cortex, 74, 107-117. [24] Schneider, K. A., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Components of visual prior entry. Cognitive Psychology, 47(4), 333-366. [25] Shore D. I., Spence C., & Klein R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry. Psychological Science, 12(3), 205-212. [26] Vatakis, A., & Spence, C. (2007). Crossmodal binding: Evaluating the "unity assumption" using audiovisual speech stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(5), 744-756. [27] Vecera, S. P., & Behrmann, M. (2001). Attention and unit formation: A biased competition account of object-based attention. Advances in Psychology, 130, 145-180. [28] Vibell J., Klinge C., Zampini M., Spence C., & Nobre A. C. (2007). Temporal order is coded temporally in the brain: Early event-related potential latency shifts underlying prior entry in a cross-modal temporal order judgment task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(1), 109-120. [29] Yates, M. J., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2009). Somatosensory prior entry. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71(4), 847-859. [30] Yun X. Y., Li W., Qiu J., Jou J., Wei D. T., Tu S., & Zhang Q. L. (2011). Neural mechanisms of subliminal priming for traumatic episodic memory: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 498(1), 10-14. [31] Zackon D. H., Casson E. J., Zafar A., Stelmach L., & Racette L. (1999). The temporal order judgment paradigm: Subcortical attentional contribution under exogenous and endogenous cueing conditions. Neuropsychologia, 37(5), 511-520. |