[1] 陈建新, 伍莉, 黄蓉, 王喆, 陈悦, 杨伟平. (2020). 情绪的相容性对创造性思维的影响. 心理与行为研究, 18(4), 433-439. [2] 胡卫平, 俞国良. (2002). 青少年的科学创造力研究. 教育研究, 1, 44-48. [3] 马谐, 白学军, 陶云. (2013). 音乐与情绪诱发的机制模型. 心理科学进展, 21(4), 643-652. [4] 孙国江. (2012). 创造发明中有无样例对发散思维的影响 (硕士学位论文). 西南大学, 重庆. [5] 杨集梅, 柴洁余, 邱天龙, 全小山, 郑茂平. (2022). 共情与中国民族音乐情绪识别的关系: 来自ERP的证据. 心理学报, 54(10), 1181-1192. [6] 姚海娟, 陈雅靖, 张云平. (2018). 情绪状态与背景音乐对创造性思维的影响. 心理研究, 11(3), 243-249. [7] 张航, 孟乐, 张积家. (2022). 为什么和声能诱发音乐情感?——音乐协和性的作用及其认知机制. 心理科学进展, 30(4), 817-833. [8] 朱海雪, 杨春娟, 李文福, 刘鑫, 邱江, 张庆林. (2012). 问题解决中顿悟的原型位置效应的fMRI研究. 心理学报, 44(8), 1025-1037. [9] Benedek M., Jauk E., Sommer M., Arendasy M., & Neubauer A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73-83. [10] Bramley S., Dibben N., & Rowe R. (2016). Investigating the influence of music tempo on arousal and behaviour in laboratory virtual roulette. Psychology of Music, 44(6), 1389-1403. [11] de la Mora Velasco, E., & Hirumi, A. (2020). The effects of background music on learning: A systematic review of literature to guide future research and practice. Educational Technology Research and Development, 68(6), 2817-2837. [12] de Vries, H. B., & Lubart, T. I. (2019). Scientific creativity: Divergent and convergent thinking and the impact of culture. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(2), 145-155. [13] Doyle, M., & Furnham, A. (2012). The distracting effects of music on the cognitive test performance of creative and non-creative individuals. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7(1), 1-7. [14] Gajda A., Karwowski M., & Beghetto R. A. (2017). Creativity and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(2), 269-299. [15] Gilhooly K. J., Ball L. J., & Macchi L. (2015). Insight and creative thinking processes: Routine and special. Thinking and Reasoning, 21(1), 1-4. [16] Hosseini S., Deng X. Q., Miyake Y., & Nozawa T. (2019). Head movement synchrony and idea generation interference - investigating background music effects on group creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2577. [17] Hu, W. P., & Adey, P. (2002). A scientific creativity test for secondary school students. International Journal of Science Education, 24(4), 389-403. [18] Ilie, G., & Thompson, W. F. (2011). Experiential and cognitive changes following seven minutes exposure to music and speech. Music Perception, 28(3), 247-264. [19] Jenkins, J. S. (2001). The Mozart effect. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 94(4), 170-172. [20] Kahneman, D. (2000). A psychological point of view: Violations of rational rules as a diagnostic of mental processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 681-683. [21] Kämpfe J., Sedlmeier P., & Renkewitz F. (2011). The impact of background music on adult listeners: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Music, 39(4), 424-448. [22] Lange E. B., Pieczykolan A., Trukenbrod H. A., & Huestegge L. (2018). The rhythm of cognition-Effects of an auditory beat on oculomotor control in reading and sequential scanning. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 11(2), 9. [23] Lehmann, J. A. M., & Seufert, T. (2017). The influence of background music on learning in the light of different theoretical perspectives and the role of working memory capacity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1902. [24] Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69(3), 220. [25] Rey, G. D. (2012). A review of research and a meta-analysis of the seductive detail effect. Educational Research Review, 7(3), 216-237. [26] Ritter, S. M., & Ferguson, S. (2017). Happy creativity: Listening to happy music facilitates divergent thinking. PLoS ONE, 12(9), e0182210. [27] Russell J. A., Weiss A., & Mendelsohn G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 493-502. [28] Stull A. T., Fiorella L., Gainer M. J., & Mayer R. E. (2018). Using transparent whiteboards to boost learning from online STEM lectures. Computers and Education, 120, 146-159. [29] Thompson W. F., Schellenberg E. G., & Husain G. (2001). Arousal, mood, and the Mozart effect. Psychological Science, 12(3), 248-251. [30] Threadgold E., Marsh J. E., McLatchie N., & Ball L. J. (2019). Background music stints creativity: Evidence from compound remote associate tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33(5), 873-888. [31] Tillmann, B. (2007). Music cognition: Learning, perception, expectations. In 4th International Symposium on Computer Music Modeling and Retrieval. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. [32] Toplyn, G., & Maguire, W. (1991). The differential effect of noise on creative task performance. Creativity Research Journal, 4(4), 337-347. [33] Xia T. S., Sun Y. Q., An Y., & Li L. L. (2023). The influence of music environment on conceptual design creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1052257. [34] Yang W. J., Green A. E., Chen Q. L., Kenett Y. N., Sun J. Z., Wei D. T., & Qiu J. (2022). Creative problem solving in knowledge-rich contexts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(10), 849-859. [35] Zabelina D., Saporta A., & Beeman M. (2016). Flexible or leaky attention in creative people? Distinct patterns of attention for different types of creative thinking. Memory and Cognition, 44(3), 488-498. [36] Zabelina D. L., O’Leary D., Pornpattananangkul N., Nusslock R., & Beeman M. (2015). Creativity and sensory gating indexed by the P50: Selective versus leaky sensory gating in divergent thinkers and creative achievers. Neuropsychologia, 69, 77-84. |