[1] 白露, 马慧, 黄宇霞, 罗跃嘉. (2005). 中国情绪图片系统的编制——在46名中国大学生中的试用. 中国心理卫生杂志, 19(11), 719-722. [2] 袁加锦, 张祎程, 陈圣栋, 罗利, 茹怡珊. (2021). 中国情绪调节词语库的初步编制与试用. 心理学报, 53(5), 445-455. [3] 张卫东, 刁静, Schick, C. J. (2004). 正、负性情绪的跨文化心理测量: PANAS维度结构检验. 心理科学, 27(1), 77-79. [4] Bai Y., Leib A. Y., Puri A. M., Whitney D., & Peng K. P. (2015). Gender differences in crowd perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1300. [5] Balcetis, E. (2016). Approach and avoidance as organizing structures for motivated distance perception. Emotion Review, 8(2), 115-128. [6] Dodgson, D. B., & Raymond, J. E. (2020). Value associations bias ensemble perception. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 82(1), 109-117. [7] Elias E., Dyer M., & Sweeny T. D. (2016). Ensemble perception of dynamic emotional groups. Psychological Science, 28(2), 193-203. [8] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [9] Fini C., Fischer M., Bardi L., Brass M., & Moors A. (2020). Support from a TMS/MEP study for a direct link between positive/negative stimuli and approach/avoidance tendencies. Neuropsychologia, 143, Article 107496. [10] Givon E., Berkovich R., Oz-Cohen E., Rubinstein K., Singer-Landau E., Udelsman-Danieli G., & Meiran N. (2023). Are women truly “more emotional” than men? sex differences in an indirect model-based measure of emotional feelings. Current Psychology, 42(36), 32469-32482. [11] Haberman, J., & Whitney, D. (2009). Seeing the mean: Ensemble coding for sets of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 718-734. [12] Im H. Y., Albohn D. N., Steiner T. G., Cushing C. A., Adams R. B. Jr., & Kveraga K. (2017). Differential hemispheric and visual stream contributions to ensemble coding of crowd emotion. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(11), 828-842. [13] Jia L., Cheng M. R., Lu J. H., Wu Y. P., & Wang J. (2023). Context consistency improves ensemble perception of facial expressions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 30(1), 280-290. [14] Kozlik J., Neumann R., & Lozo L. (2015). Contrasting motivational orientation and evaluative coding accounts: On the need to differentiate the effectors of approach/avoidance responses. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 563. [15] Lakens, D., & Caldwell, A. R. (2021). Simulation-Based power analysis for factorial analysis of variance designs. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 4(1), Article 2515245920951503. [16] Lang P. J., Bradley M. M., & Cuthbert B. N. (2001). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings.University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology. [17] Munneke J., Duymaz İ., & Corbett J. E. (2022). Value-driven effects on perceptual averaging. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 84(3), 781-794. [18] Neumann, R., & Schneider, L. J. (2024). What is in a smile: The role of evaluation goal and response labels in facial muscle responses to prejudiced groups. Psychophysiology, 61(3), Article e14518. [19] Robertd T., Cant J. S., & Nestor A. (2019). Elucidating the neural representation and the processing dynamics of face ensembles. Journal of Neuroscience, 39(39), 7737-7747. [20] Roskes M., Elliot A. J., Nijstad B. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2013). Avoidance motivation and conservation of energy. Emotion Review, 5(3), 264-268. [21] Scherer, K. R., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: Event appraisal and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 719-745. [22] Shahane A. D., Lopez R. B., & Denny B. T. (2019). Implicit reappraisal as an emotional buffer: Reappraisal-related neural activity moderates the relationship between inattention and perceived stress during exposure to negative stimuli. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(2), 355-365. [23] Stevens, J. S., & Hamann, S. (2012). Sex differences in brain activation to emotional stimuli: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1578-1593. [24] Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220-247. [25] Tottenham N., Tanaka J. W., Leon A. C., McCarry T., Nurse M., Hare T. A., & Nelson C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242-249. [26] van Peer J. M., Rotteveel M., Spinhoven P., Tollenaar M. S., & Roelofs K. (2010). Affect-congruent approach and withdrawal movements of happy and angry faces facilitate affective categorisation. Cognition and Emotion, 24(5), 863-875. [27] Wang H. Y., Xu G. Q., Ni M. F., Zhang C. H., Sun X. P., Chang Y., & Zhang B. W. (2017). Neural mechanisms of implicit cognitive reappraisal: Preceding descriptions alter emotional response to unpleasant images. Neuroscience, 347, 65-75. [28] Watson D., Clark L. A., & Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070. [29] Whitney, D., & Yamanashi Leib, A. (2018). Ensemble perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(1), 105-129. [30] Wolz I., Nannt J., & Svaldi J. (2020). Laboratory-based interventions targeting food craving: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews, 21(5), e12996. [31] Wong S. F., Krause S., Marishel D., & Grisham J. R. (2021). Reappraisal of disgust: Self-report and behavioural assessment of individuals with moderate to high contamination fears. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 78, 102346. [32] Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 3-47. |