[1] 陈洁佳, 周翊, 陈杰. (2020). 音乐训练与抑制控制的关系: 来自ERPs的证据. 心理学报, 52(12), 1365-1376. [2] 何怡娟, 胡馨木, 买晓琴. (2022). 共情关怀对公平决策的影响——来自ERP的证据. 心理学报, 54(4), 385-397. [3] 辛勇, 李红, 袁加锦. (2010). 负性情绪干扰行为抑制控制: 一项事件相关电位研究. 心理学报, 42(3), 334-341. [4] 杨丽珠, 陈靖涵, 王素霞, 沈悦, 孙岩. (2017). 内生意图性抑制: 自我控制研究的新视角. 心理科学进展, 25(10), 1631-1641. [5] 袁加锦, 徐萌萌, 杨洁敏, 李红. (2017). 双选择Oddball范式在行为抑制控制研究中的应用. 中国科学: 生命科学, 47(10), 1065-1073. [6] Buodo G., Sarlo M., Mento G., Messerotti Benvenuti S., & Palomba D. (2017). Unpleasant stimuli differentially modulate inhibitory processes in an emotional Go/No-go task: An event-related potential study. Cognition and Emotion, 31(1), 127-138. [7] Cohen, M. X. (2017). Where does EEG come from and what does it mean? Trends in Neurosciences, 40(4), 208-218. [8] Dall’Acqua T., Begliomini C., Motta R., Miotto D., & Castiello U. (2018). Effects of intentionality and subliminal information in free-choices to inhibit. Neuropsychologia, 109, 28-38. [9] Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9-21. [10] Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168. [11] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [12] Filevich E., Kühn S., & Haggard P. (2012). Intentional inhibition in human action: The power of 'no'. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1107-1118. [13] Fogarty J. S., Barry R. J., & Steiner G. Z. (2019). Sequential processing in the classic oddball task: ERP components, probability, and behavior. Psychophysiology, 56(3), Article e13300. [14] Hennecke, M., & Bürgler, S. (2023). Metacognition and self-control: An integrative framework. Psychological Review, 130(5), 1262-1288. [15] Huster R. J., Enriquez-Geppert S., Lavallee C. F., Falkenstein M., & Herrmann C. S. (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks and cognitive contributions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(3), 217-233. [16] Inzlicht M., Werner K. M., Briskin J. L., & Roberts B. W. (2021). Integrating models of self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 319-345. [17] Kühn S., Haggard P., & Brass M. (2009). Intentional inhibition: How the "veto-area" exerts control. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2834-2843. [18] Liu X. Y., Liu Y., Song S. Q., Xiang G. C., Du X. L., Li Q. Q., & Chen H. (2022). "Free won't" of food in overweight and normal-weight adults: Comparison of neurocognitive correlates of intentional and reactive inhibitions. Neuropsychologia, 174, Article 108351. [19] Lynn M. T., Muhle-Karbe P. S., & Brass M. (2014). Controlling the self: The role of the dorsal frontomedian cortex in intentional inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 247-254. [20] Parkinson, J., & Haggard, P. (2015). Choosing to stop: Responses evoked by externally triggered and internally generated inhibition identify a neural mechanism of will. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(10), 1948-1956. [21] Rae C. L., Ahmad A., Larsson D. E. O., Silva M., van Praag, C. D. G., Garfinkel S. N., & Critchley H. D. (2020). Impact of cardiac interoception cues and confidence on voluntary decisions to make or withhold action in an intentional inhibition task. Scientific Reports, 10(1), Article 4184. [22] Ridderinkhof K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Brass M. (2014). "Don' t" versus" "Won' T": Principles, mechanisms, and intention in action inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 255-262. [23] Schel M. A., Scheres A., & Crone E. A. (2014). New perspectives on self-control development: Highlighting the role of intentional inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 236-246. [24] Shen Y., Zhao H., Zhu J. Y., He Y., Zhang X., Liu S. H., & Chen J. H. (2020). Comparison of intentional inhibition and reactive inhibition in adolescents and adults: An ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 45(2), 66-78. [25] Wang Y. C., Liu P., Yao Z., & Wang Y. H. (2020). Why do you often stop your impulsive behavior at the last moment? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27(3), 498-503. [26] Weidacker K., Kvamme T. L., Whiteford S., Valle Guzman N., & Voon V. (2021). Incentives and voluntary stopping: The intentional hand task. Cognition, 206, Article 104504. [27] Xu M. S., Fan L. X., Li Z. A., Qi S. Q., & Yang D. (2020). Neural signatures of reactive and intentional inhibitions: An ERP study. Current Psychology, 39(5), 1485-1495. [28] Xu M. S., Li Z. A., Qi S. Q., Fan L. X., Zhou X. L., & Yang D. (2020). Social exclusion modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Human Brain Mapping, 41(10), 2669-2685. [29] Xu M. S., Wen J. Y., Li Z. A., Wang Z. H., & Zhang J. H. (2024). Behavioral impulse and time pressure jointly influence intentional inhibition: Evidence from the free two-choice oddball task. Psychological Research, 88(3), 936-949. [30] Zhang G. H., Li X. Y., & Cong F. Y. (2020). Objective extraction of evoked event-related oscillation from time-frequency representation of event-related potentials. Neural Plasticity, 2020(1), Article 8841354. |