Journal of Psychological Science ›› 2025, Vol. 48 ›› Issue (2): 280-294.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20250203
• General Psychology, Experimental Psychology & Ergonomics • Previous Articles Next Articles
Xing Liangyu1,2, Yu Tingting1, Qiao Xinuo1, Hao Ning1,3
Online:
2025-03-20
Published:
2025-04-21
邢梁玉1,2, 于婷婷1, 乔熙诺1, 郝宁**1,3
通讯作者:
**郝宁,E-mail: nhao@psy.ecnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
Xing Liangyu, Yu Tingting, Qiao Xinuo, Hao Ning. Sniffing Out Originality? The Influence of Odors on the Evaluation of Creative Ideas[J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2025, 48(2): 280-294.
邢梁玉, 于婷婷, 乔熙诺, 郝宁. 嗅出新颖性?气味对创造性观点评价的影响*[J]. 心理科学, 2025, 48(2): 280-294.
[1] 王娟, 沈树华, 张积家. (2011). 大学生的气味词分类——基于语义相似性和知觉相似性的探讨. 心理学报, 43(10), 1124-1137. [2] 赵庆柏, 柯娓, 童彪, 周治金, 周宗奎. (2017). 网络语言的创造性加工过程: 新颖N400与LPC. 心理学报, 49(2), 143-154. [3] 周雯, 冯果. (2012). 嗅知觉及其与情绪系统的交互. 心理科学进展, 20(1), 2-9. [4] 周晓林, 叶铮. (2006). 语言认知的脑基础. 见罗跃嘉, 姜扬, 程康 (编). 认知神经科学教程 (pp. 300-313). 北京大学出版社. [5] Abraham A., Rutter B., & Hermann C. (2021). Conceptual expansion via novel metaphor processing: An ERP replication and extension study examining individual differences in creativity. Brain and Language, 221, Article 105007. [6] Achour M., Salem I., B. Ferdousi F., Nouira M., Fredj M., B. Mtiraoui A., Isoda H., & Saguem S. (2022). Rosemary tea consumption alters peripheral anxiety and depression biomarkers: A pilot study in limited healthy volunteers. Journal of the American Nutrition Association, 41(3), 240-249. [7] Angelucci F. L., Silva V. V., Dal Pizzol C., Spir L. G., Praes C. E. O., & Maibach H. (2014). Physiological effect of olfactory stimuli inhalation in humans: An overview. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 36(2), 117-123. [8] Araki R., Sasaki K., Onda H., Nakamura S., & Hashimoto K. (2020). Effects of continuous intake of rosemary extracts on mental health in working generation healthy Japanese men: Post-hoc testing of a randomized controlled trial. Nutrients, 12(11), Article 3551. [9] Arzouan Y., Goldstein A., & Faust M. (2007). Brainwaves are stethoscopes: ERP correlates of novel metaphor comprehension. Brain Research, 1160, 69-81. [10] Ashby F. G., Isen A. M., & Turken A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106(3), 529-550. [11] Atsumi, T., & Tonosaki, K. (2007). Smelling lavender and rosemary increases free radical scavenging activity and decreases cortisol level in saliva. Psychiatry Research, 150(1), 89-96. [12] Ballanger B., Bath K. G., & Mandairon N. (2019). Odorants: A tool to provide nonpharmacological intervention to reduce anxiety during normal and pathological aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 82, 18-29. [13] Baron, R. A. (1983). “Sweet smell of success”? The impact of pleasant artificial scents on evaluations of job applicants. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68(4), 709-713. [14] Baron, R. A., & Bronfen, M. I. (1994). A whiff of reality: Empirical evidence concerning the effects of pleasant fragrances on work-related behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(13), 1179-1203. [15] Baron, R. A., & Kalsher, M. J. (1998). Effects of a pleasant ambient fragrance on simulated driving performance: The sweet smell of.. safety? Environment and Behavior, 30(4), 535-552. [16] Baron, R. A., & Thomley, J. (1994). A whiff of reality: Positive affect as a potential mediator of the effects of pleasant fragrances on task performance and helping. Environment and Behavior, 26(6), 766-784. [17] Bauer, P. J., & Jackson, F. L. (2015). Semantic elaboration: ERPs reveal rapid transition from novel to known. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(1), 271-282. [18] Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59. [19] Burnett K. M., Solterbeck L. A., & Strapp C. M. (2004). Scent and mood state following an anxiety-provoking task. Psychological Reports, 95(2), 707-722. [20] Busca L., Grobert J., & Vellera C. (2021). Effect of congruent scent diffusion on individual creative fluidity: Mental imagery instruction and iconic stimulus as possible moderators. Creativity and Innovation Management, 30(3), 511-522. [21] Cahill L., Babinsky R., Markowitsch H. J., & McGaugh J. L. (1995). The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature, 377(6547), 295-296. [22] Campenni C. E., Crawley E. J., & Meier M. E. (2004). Role of suggestion in odor-induced mood change. Psychological Reports, 94(3), 1127-1136. [23] Carnevale, P. J., & Probst, T. M. (1998). Social values and social conflict in creative problem solving and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1300-1309. [24] Chebat J. C., Chebat C. G., & Vaillant D. (2001). Environmental background music and in-store selling. Journal of Business Research, 54(2), 115-123. [25] Chebat, J. C., & Michon, R. (2003). Impact of ambient odors on mall shoppers'emotions, cognition, and spending: A test of competitive causal theories. Journal of Business Research, 56(7), 529-539. [26] Chioca L. R., Ferro M. M., Baretta I. P., Oliveira S. M., Silva C. R., Ferreira J., Losso E. M., & Andreatini R. (2013). Anxiolytic-like effect of lavender essential oil inhalation in mice: Participation of serotonergic but not GABAA/benzodiazepine neurotransmission. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 147(2), 412-418. [27] Dalton P., Maute C., Oshida A., Hikichi S., & Izumi Y. (2008). The use of semantic differential scaling to define the multidimensional representation of odors. Journal of Sensory Studies, 23(4), 485-497. [28] De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas M., & Nijstad B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 739-756. [29] Diego M. A., Jones N. A., Field T., Hernandez-Reif M., Schanberg S., Kuhn C., & Galamaga M. (1998). Aromatherapy positively affects mood, EEG patterns of alertness and math computations. International Journal of Neuroscience, 96(3-4), 217-224. [30] Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(6), 1011-1026. [31] Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 822-848. [32] Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298(5596), 1191-1194. [33] Eichenbaum, H. (2001). The hippocampus and declarative memory: Cognitive mechanisms and neural codes. Behavioural Brain Research, 127(1-2), 199-207. [34] Ellen, P. S., & Bone, P. F. (1998). Does it matter if it smells? Olfactory stimuli as advertising executional cues. Journal of Advertising, 27(4), 29-39. [35] Filiptsova O. V., Gazzavi-Rogozina L. V., Timoshyna I. A., Naboka O. I., Dyomina Y. V., & Ochkur A. V. (2018). The effect of the essential oils of lavender and rosemary on the human short-term memory. Alexandria Journal of Medicine, 54(1), 41-44. [36] Finke, R. A., & Slayton, K. (1988). Explorations of creative visual synthesis in mental imagery. Memory and Cognition, 16(3), 252-257. [37] Gao Z. N., Cheng L. L., Li J. C., Chen Q. L., & Hao N. (2022). The dark side of creativity: Neural correlates of malevolent creative idea generation. Neuropsychologia, 167, Article 108164. [38] George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1996). Motivational agendas in the workplace: The effects of feelings on focus of attention and work motivation. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior: An annual series of analytical essays and critical reviews (pp. 75-109). Elsevier Science. [39] Gonçalves F., Cabral D., Campos P., & Schöning J. (2017). I smell creativity: Exploring the effects of olfactory and auditory cues to support creative writing Tasks. In Proceedings of the 16th IFIP TC 13 International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, Cham. [40] Guilford, J. P. (1967). Creativity: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 1(1), 3-14. [41] Hagoort, P. (2003). How the brain solves the binding problem for language: A neurocomputational model of syntactic processing. NeuroImage, 20, S18-S29. [42] Hagoort P., Hald L., Bastiaansen M., & Petersson K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438-441. [43] Hasselmo, M. E., & Sarter, M. (2011). Modes and models of forebrain cholinergic neuromodulation of cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1), 52-73. [44] Hayes A. F.(2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press. [45] Herz, R. S. (2009). Aromatherapy facts and fictions: A scientific analysis of olfactory effects on mood, physiology and behavior. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(2), 263-290. [46] Ho, C., & Spence, C. (2005). Olfactory facilitation of dual-task performance. Neuroscience Letters, 389(1), 35-40. [47] Hussain S. M., Syeda A. F., Alshammari M., Alnasser S., Alenzi N. D., Alanazi S. T., & Nandakumar K. (2022). Cognition enhancing effect of rosemary (Rosmarinus officinalis L.) in lab animal studies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 55, Article e11593. [48] Iijima M., Nio E., Nashimoto E., & Iwata M. (2007). Effects of aroma on the autonomic nervous system and brain activity under stress conditions. Autonomic Neuroscience, 135(1-2), 97-98. [49] Ivcevic Z.,& Hoffmann, J. (2019). Emotions and creativity: From process to person and product. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity ( pp. 273-295). Cambridge University Press. [50] Jia L. J., Tang S., Song Y., Ren J. Y., Li F. H., & Huang F. R. (2023). Cognitive control in creative discovery: The gap between ideal and reality. Biological Psychology, 178, Article 108522. [51] Kennedy D. O., Dodd F. L., Robertson B. C., Okello E. J., Reay J. L., Scholey A. B., & Haskell C. F. (2011). Monoterpenoid extract of sage (Salvia lavandulaefolia) with cholinesterase inhibiting properties improves cognitive performance and mood in healthy adults. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25(8), 1088-1100. [52] Krämer, W. (2014). Thinking, fast and slow. Statistical Papers, 55(3), 915. [53] Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470. [54] Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words. Biological Psychology, 11(2), 99-116. [55] Kutas M.,& van Petten, C. K. (1994). Psycholinguistics electrified: Event-related brain potential investigations In M A Gernsbacher (Ed), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp 83-143) San Diego: Academic Press Event-related brain potential investigations. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 83-143). San Diego: Academic Press. [56] Kutlu A. K., Yılmaz E., & Çeçen D. (2008). Effects of aroma inhalation on examination anxiety. Teaching and Learning in Nursing, 3(4), 125-130. [57] Larsson, M., & Willander, J. (2009). Autobiographical odor memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1170(1), 318-323. [58] Lee Y. S., Chang J. Y., & Choi J. N. (2017). Why reject creative ideas? Fear as a driver of implicit bias against creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 225-235. [59] Lorig T. S., Huffman E., DeMartino A., & DeMarco J. (1991). The effects of low concentration odors on EEG activity and behavior. Journal of Psychophysiology, 5(1), 69-77. [60] Lorig T. S., Schwartz G. E., Herman K. B., & Lane R. D. (1988). Brain and odor: IL EEG activity during nose and mouth breathing. Psychobiology, 16(3), 285-287. [61] Lu, K. L., & Hao, N. (2019). When do we fall in neural synchrony with others? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(3), 253-261. [62] Lu K. L., Qiao X. N., & Hao N. (2019). Praising or keeping silent on partner's ideas: Leading brainstorming in particular ways. Neuropsychologia, 124, 19-30. [63] Luo J., Niki K., & Knoblich G. (2006). Perceptual contributions to problem solving: Chunk decomposition of Chinese characters. Brain Research Bulletin, 70(4-6), 430-443. [64] Mastria S., Agnoli S., & Corazza G. E. (2019). How does emotion influence the creativity evaluation of exogenous alternative ideas? PLoS ONE, 14(7), Article e0219298. [65] Morrin, M., & Ratneshwar, S. (2000). The impact of ambient scent on evaluation, attention, and memory for familiar and unfamiliar brands. Journal of Business Research, 49(2), 157-165. [66] Moss M., Cook J., Wesnes K., & Duckett P. (2003). Aromas of rosemary and lavender essential oils differentially affect cognition and mood in healthy adults. International Journal of Neuroscience, 113(1), 15-38. [67] Moss, M., & Oliver, L. (2012). Plasma 1, 8-cineole correlates with cognitive performance following exposure to rosemary essential oil aroma. Therapeutic Advances in Psychopharmacology, 2(3), 103-113. [68] Moss M., Smith E., Milner M., & McCready J. (2018). Acute ingestion of rosemary water: Evidence of cognitive and cerebrovascular effects in healthy adults. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(12), 1319-1329. [69] Mumford, M. D. (2003). Where have we been, where are we going? Taking stock in creativity research. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2-3), 107-120. [70] Neville K. R.,& Haberly, L. B. (2004). Olfactory cortex. In G. M. Shepherd (Ed.), The synaptic organization of the brain (pp. 415-454). Oxford University Press. [71] Orwig W., Beaty R. E., Benedek M., & Schacter, D. L.(in press). Creative evaluation: The role of memory in novelty & effectiveness judgements. Creativity Research Journal. [72] Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1993). Event-related potentials and syntactic anomaly: Evidence of anomaly detection during the perception of continuous speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 413-437. [73] Osterhout, L., & Nicol, J. (1999). On the distinctiveness, independence, and time course of the brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(3), 283-317. [74] Patterson, F. (2004). Personal initiative and innovation. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Encyclopedia of applied psychology (pp. 843-855). Elsevier. [75] Pellegrino R., Sinding C., de Wijk R. A., & Hummel T. (2017). Habituation and adaptation to odors in humans. Physiology and Behavior, 177, 13-19. [76] Pijnacker J., Geurts B., van Lambalgen M., Buitelaar J., & Hagoort P. (2011). Reasoning with exceptions: An event-related brain potentials study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 471-480. [77] Rataj K., Przekoracka-Krawczyk A., & van der Lubbe, R. H. J. (2018). On understanding creative language: The late positive complex and novel metaphor comprehension. Brain Research, 1678, 231-244. [78] Razumnikova, O. M. (2009). Peculiarities of information selection in the process of creative thinking. Psychology Journal of the Higher School of Economics, 6, 134-161. [79] Riby L. M., Edwards S., McDonald H., & Moss M. (2023). The impact of a rosemary containing drink on event-related potential neural markers of sustained attention. PLoS ONE, 18(6), Article e0286113. [80] Rietzschel E. F., Nijstad B. A., & Stroebe W. (2010). The selection of creative ideas after individual idea generation: Choosing between creativity and impact. British Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 47-68. [81] Ritter S. M., Strick M., Bos M. W., van Baaren R. B., & Dijksterhuis A. (2012). Good morning creativity: Task reactivation during sleep enhances beneficial effect of sleep on creative performance. Journal of Sleep Research, 21(6), 643-647. [82] Rominger C., Benedek M., Lebuda I., Perchtold-Stefan C. M., Schwerdtfeger A. R., Papousek I., & Fink A. (2022). Functional brain activation patterns of creative metacognitive monitoring. Neuropsychologia, 177, Article 108416. [83] Rösler, F., & Heil, M. (1991). Toward a functional categorization of slow waves: Taking into account past and future events. Psychophysiology, 28(3), 344-358. [84] Runco M. A., Plucker J. A., & Lim W. (2001). Development and psychometric integrity of a measure of ideational behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3-4), 393-400. [85] Rutter B., Kröger S., Hill H., Windmann S., Hermann C., & Abraham A. (2012). Can clouds dance? Part 2: An ERP investigation of passive conceptual expansion. Brain and Cognition, 80(3), 301-310. [86] Sayorwan W., Siripornpanich V., Piriyapunyaporn T., Hongratanaworakit T., Kotchabhakdi N., & Ruangrungsi N. (2012). The effects of lavender oil inhalation on emotional states, autonomic nervous system, and brain electrical activity. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand, 95(4), 598-606. [87] Scheydt S., Müller Staub M., Frauenfelder F., Nielsen G. H., Behrens J., & Needham I. (2017). Sensory overload: A concept analysis. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 26(2), 110-120. [88] Setzer, W. N. (2009). Essential oils and anxiolytic aromatherapy. Natural Product Communications, 4(9), 1305-1316. [89] Sowndhararajan K., Cho H., Yu B., & Kim S. (2015). Effect of olfactory stimulation of isomeric aroma compounds, (+)-limonene and terpinolene on human electroencephalographic activity. European Journal of Integrative Medicine, 7(6), 561-566. [90] Sowndhararajan, K., & Kim, S. (2016). Influence of fragrances on human psychophysiological [91] activity: With special reference to human electroencephalographic response. Scientia Pharmaceutica, 84(4), 724-751. [92] Spangenberg E. R., Crowley A. E., & Henderson P. W. (1996). Improving the store environment: Do olfactory cues affect evaluations and behaviors? Journal of Marketing, 60(2), 67-80. [93] Tang S., Jia L. J., Ren J. Y., Li F. H., Luo J., & Huang F. R. (2022). Reactive control in evaluating appropriately or inappropriately novel ideas: Evidence from electrophysiological measures. Psychophysiology, 59(7), Article e14010. [94] Touhara, K., & Vosshall, L. B. (2009). Sensing odorants and pheromones with chemosensory receptors. Annual Review of Physiology, 71, 307-332. [95] Valnet, J. (1990). The practice of aromatherapy. Rochester: Healing Arts Press. [96] Vernet-Maury E., Alaoui-Ismaïli O., Dittmar A., Delhomme G., & Chanel J. (1999). Basic emotions induced by odorants: A new approach based on autonomic pattern results. Journal of the Autonomic Nervous System, 75(2-3), 176-183. [97] Wang B. T., Zhou H., Duan H. J., Wang X. W., Song B. P., & Hu W. P. (2021). Highly creative individuals process creative pictorial information more effectively: Evidence from an ERP Study. Creativity Research Journal, 33(2), 124-137. [98] Warm J. S., Dember W. N., & Parasuraman R. (1991). Effects of olfactory stimulation on performance and stress in a visual sustained attention task. Journal of the Society of Cosmetic Chemists, 42, 199-210. |
Viewed | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Full text 326
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Abstract 324
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||