Journal of Psychological Science ›› 2025, Vol. 48 ›› Issue (2): 447-458.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20250217
• Social, Personality & Organizational Psychology • Previous Articles Next Articles
Li Yuchu1,2, Zhang Siqi1,2, Ding Geyi1,2, Niu Jiawen1,2, Rao Lilin1,2
Online:
2025-03-20
Published:
2025-04-21
李玉楚1,2, 张思琦1,2, 丁格一1,2, 牛佳雯1,2, 饶俪琳**1,2
通讯作者:
**饶俪琳,E-mail: raoll@psych.ac.cn
基金资助:
Li Yuchu, Zhang Siqi, Ding Geyi, Niu Jiawen, Rao Lilin. The Underlying Mechanism of Believing and Sharing of Misinformation: Based on the Tinbergen’s Framework[J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2025, 48(2): 447-458.
李玉楚, 张思琦, 丁格一, 牛佳雯, 饶俪琳. 虚假信息相信与分享的内在机制:基于廷伯根理论框架*[J]. 心理科学, 2025, 48(2): 447-458.
[1] 新华网. (202320232023). 2022年我国未成年人互联网普及率达97.2%. http://www.xinhuanet.com/politics/20231223/f7203959bf5248c68a2408a18df172bf/c.html [2] 中国网信网. (2022). 中央网信办部署开展“清朗·打击网络谣言和虚假信息”专项行动. https://www.cac.gov.cn/2022-09/02/c_1663745754062601.htm [3] Adams Z., Osman M., Bechlivanidis C., & Meder B. (2023). (Why) is misinformation a problem? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 18(6), 1436-1463. [4] Altay S., Hacquin A. S., & Mercier H. (2022). Why do so few people share fake news? It hurts their reputation. New Media and Society, 24(6), 1303-1324. [5] Arechar A. A., Allen J., Berinsky A. J., Cole R., Epstein Z., Garimella K., … Rand D. G. (2023). Understanding and combatting misinformation across 16 countries on six continents. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(9), 1502-1513. [6] Bago B., Rand D. G., & Pennycook G. (2020). Fake news, fast and slow: Deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(8), 1608-1613. [7] Balakrishnan V., Ng K. S., & Rahim H. A. (2021). To share or not to share - the underlying motives of sharing fake news amidst the COVID-19 pandemic in Malaysia. Technology in Society, 66, Article 101676. [8] Baptista J. P., Correia E., Gradim A., & Piñeiro-Naval V. (2021). The influence of political ideology on fake news belief: The Portuguese case. Publications, 9(2), Article 23. [9] Baron, J. (2019). Actively open-minded thinking in politics. Cognition, 188, 8-18. [10] Bateson, P., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Tinbergen's four questions: An appreciation and an update. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 28(12), 712-718. [11] Beauchamp, G. (2010). Determinants of false alarms in staging flocks of semipalmated sandpipers. Behavioral Ecology, 21(3), 584-587. [12] Beauchamp, G., & Ruxton, G. D. (2007). False alarms and the evolution of antipredator vigilance. Animal Behaviour, 74(5), 1199-1206. [13] Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2012). What makes online content viral? Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192-205. [14] Bermes, A. (2021). Information overload and fake news sharing: A transactional stress perspective exploring the mitigating role of consumers'resilience during COVID-19. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 61, Article 102555. [15] Brashier, N. M., & Marsh, E. J. (2020). Judging truth. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 499-515. [16] Brashier, N. M., & Schacter, D. L. (2020). Aging in an era of fake news. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(3), 316-323. [17] Brashier N. M., Umanath S., Cabeza R., & Marsh E. J. (2017). Competing cues: Older adults rely on knowledge in the face of fluency. Psychology and Aging, 32(4), 331-337. [18] Breakstone J., Smith M., Wineburg S., Rapaport A., Carle J., Garland M., & Saavedra A. (2021). Students'civic online reasoning: A national portrait. Educational Researcher, 50(8), 505-515. [19] Bronstein M. V., Pennycook G., Buonomano L., & Cannon T. D. (2021). Belief in fake news, responsiveness to cognitive conflict, and analytic reasoning engagement. Thinking and Reasoning, 27(4), 510-535. [20] Buss D. M., Haselton M. G., Shackelford T. K., Bleske A. L., & Wakefield J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist, 53(5), 533-548. [21] Cantlon, J. F., & Piantadosi, S. T. (2024). Uniquely human intelligence arose from expanded information capacity. Nature Reviews Psychology, 3(4), 275-293. [22] Ceylan G., Anderson I. A., & Wood W. (2023). Sharing of misinformation is habitual, not just lazy or biased. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 120(4), Article e2216614120. [23] Cushman, F. (2020). Rationalization is rational. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, Article e28. [24] Danovitch, J. H., & Lane, J. D. (2020). Children's belief in purported events: When claims reference hearsay, books, or the internet. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 193, Article 104808. [25] Durante, K. M., & Griskevicius, V. (2018). Evolution and consumer psychology. Consumer Psychology Review, 1(1), 4-21. [26] Ekstrom, P. D., & Lai, C. K. (2021). The selective communication of political information. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(5), 789-800. [27] Fahimipour A. K., Gil M. A., Celis M. R., Hein G. F., Martin B. T., & Hein A. M. (2023). Wild animals suppress the spread of socially transmitted misinformation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 120(14), Article e2215428120. [28] Forgas, J. P. (2019). Happy believers and sad skeptics? Affective influences on gullibility. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 306-313. [29] Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25-42. [30] Garrett, R. K., & Bond, R. M. (2021). Conservatives'susceptibility to political misperceptions. Science Advances, 7(23), Article eabf1234. [31] Girouard-Hallam L. N., Tong Y., Wang F. X., & Danovitch J. (2023). What can the internet do? Chinese and American children's attitudes and beliefs about the internet. Cognitive Development, 66, Article 101338. [32] Gray, L., & Webster, M. M. (2023). False alarms and information transmission in grouping animals. Biological Reviews, 98(3), 833-848. [33] Grinberg N., Joseph K., Friedland L., Swire-Thompson B., & Lazer D. (2019). Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Science, 363(6425), 374-378. [34] Hargittai, E., & Dobransky, K. (2017). Old dogs, new clicks: Digital inequality in skills and uses among older adults. Canadian Journal of Communication, 42(2), 195-212. [35] Hargittai E., Piper A. M., & Morris M. R. (2019). From internet access to internet skills: Digital inequality among older adults. Universal Access in the Information Society, 18(4), 881-890. [36] Islam A. K. M. N., Laato S., Talukder S., & Sutinen E. (2020). Misinformation sharing and social media fatigue during COVID-19: An affordance and cognitive load perspective. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 159, Article 120201. [37] Islam A. K. M. N., Mäntymäki M., & Benbasat I. (2019). Duality of self-promotion on social networking sites. Information Technology and People, 32(2), 269-296. [38] Iyengar, S., & Massey, D. S. (2019). Scientific communication in a post-truth society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(16), 7656-7661. [39] Johnstone, R. A., & Manica, A. (2011). Evolution of personality differences in leadership. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(20), 8373-8378. [40] Jones-Jang S. M., Mortensen T., & Liu J. (2021). Does media literacy help identification of fake news? Information literacy helps, but other literacies don't. American Behavioral Scientist, 65(2), 371-388. [41] Kahan, D. M. (2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(4), 407-424. [42] Kahan D. M., Peters E., Dawson E. C., & Slovic P. (2017). Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government. Behavioural Public Policy, 1(1), 54-86. [43] Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux. [44] Koch, A. S., & Forgas, J. P. (2012). Feeling good and feeling truth: The interactive effects of mood and processing fluency on truth judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 481-485. [45] Kozhevnikov, M. (2007). Cognitive styles in the context of modern psychology: Toward an integrated framework of cognitive style. Psychological Bulletin, 133(3), 464-481. [46] Laland K. N., Sterelny K., Odling-Smee J., Hoppitt W., & Uller T. (2011). Cause and effect in biology revisited: Is Mayr's proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science, 334(6062), 1512-1516. [47] Lanius C., Weber R., & MacKenzie W. I. (2021). Use of bot and content flags to limit the spread of misinformation among social networks: A behavior and attitude survey. Social Network Analysis and Mining, 11(1), Article 32. [48] Levine, T. R. (2022). Truth-default theory and the psychology of lying and deception detection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 47, Article 101380. [49] Lewis D. M. G., Al-Shawaf L., Conroy-Beam D., Asao K., & Buss D. M. (2017). Evolutionary psychology: A how-to guide. American Psychologist, 72(4), 353-373. [50] Li M. H., Chen Z. Q., & Rao L. L. (2022). Emotion, analytic thinking and susceptibility to misinformation during the COVID-19 outbreak. Computers in Human Behavior, 133, Article 107295. [51] Liu Z. W., Zhang T. L., Yang K. L., Thompson P., Yu Z. P., & Ananiadou S. (2024). Emotion detection for misinformation: A review. Information Fusion, 107, Article 102300. [52] Loos E. F., Ivan L., & Kaufman D. (2018). “Save the pacific northwest tree octopus”: A hoax revisited. or: How vulnerable are school children to fake news? Information and Learning Science, 119(9-10), 514-528. [53] Lu X. R., Vijaykumar S., Jin Y., & Rogerson D. (2022). Think before you share: Beliefs and emotions that shaped COVID-19 (Mis)information vetting and sharing intentions among WhatsApp users in the United Kingdom. Telematics and Informatics, 67, Article 101750. [54] Martel C., Pennycook G., & Rand D. G. (2020). Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 5(1), Article 47. [55] Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 209-237. [56] Mazhar B., Niu J., Zhong Q. Y., & Haq I. U. (2024). Unraveling the dynamics of COVID-19 fake news spread: The influence of socialization, entertainment, and altruism in instant news sharing on social media. Telematics and Informatics Reports, 15, Article 100155. [57] McGrew S., Breakstone J., Ortega T., Smith M., & Wineburg S. (2018). Can students evaluate online sources? Learning from assessments of civic online reasoning. Theory and Research in Social Education, 46(2), 165-193. [58] Melchior, C., & Oliveira, M. (2024). A systematic literature review of the motivations to share fake news on social media platforms and how to fight them. New Media and Society, 26(2), 1127-1150. [59] Mirhoseini M., Early S., El Shamy N., & Hassanein K. (2023). Actively open-minded thinking is key to combating fake news: A multimethod study. Information and Management, 60(3), Article 103761. [60] Moore, R. C., & Hancock, J. T. (2022). A digital media literacy intervention for older adults improves resilience to fake news. Scientific Reports, 12(1), Article 6008. [61] Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist, 61(6), 622-631. [62] Osmundsen M., Bor A., Vahlstrup P. B., Bechmann A., & Petersen M. B. (2021). Partisan polarization is the primary psychological motivation behind political fake news sharing on Twitter. American Political Science Review, 115(3), 999-1015. [63] Panizza F., Ronzani P., Martini C., Mattavelli S., Morisseau T., & Motterlini M. (2022). Lateral reading and monetary incentives to spot disinformation about science. Scientific Reports, 12(1), Article 5678. [64] Pehlivanoglu D., Lin T., Deceus F., Heemskerk A., Ebner N. C., & Cahill B. S. (2021). The role of analytical reasoning and source credibility on the evaluation of real and fake full-length news articles. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 6(1), Article 24. [65] Pennycook G., Bear A., Collins E. T., & Rand D. G. (2020a). The implied truth effect: Attaching warnings to a subset of fake news headlines increases perceived accuracy of headlines without warnings. Management Science, 66(11), 4944-4957. [66] Pennycook G., McPhetres J., Zhang Y. H., Lu J. G., & Rand D. G. (2020b). Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: Experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychological Science, 31(7), 770-780. [67] Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019a). Fighting misinformation on social media using crowdsourced judgments of news source quality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(7), 2521-2526. [68] Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019b). Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition, 188, 39-50. [69] Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. Journal of Personality, 88(2), 185-200. [70] Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2021). The psychology of fake news. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(5), 388-402. [71] Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2022). Accuracy prompts are a replicable and generalizable approach for reducing the spread of misinformation. Nature Communications, 13(1), Article 2333. [72] Pereira A., Harris E., & van Bavel, J. J. (2023). Identity concerns drive belief: The impact of partisan identity on the belief and dissemination of true and false news. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 26(1), 24-47. [73] Rahwan I., Cebrian M., Obradovich N., Bongard J., Bonnefon J. F., Breazeal C., … Wellman M. (2019). Machine behaviour. Nature, 568(7753), 477-486. [74] Raihani, N. J., & Bell, V. (2019). An evolutionary perspective on paranoia. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 114-121. [75] Rathje S., Roozenbeek J., van Bavel J. J., & van Der Linden, S. (2023). Accuracy and social motivations shape judgements of (mis)information. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(6), 892-903. [76] Roozenbeek J., Schneider C. R., Dryhurst S., Kerr J., Freeman A. L. J., Recchia G., ..van Der Linden S. (2020). Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. Royal Society Open Science, 7(10), Article 201199. [77] Ross R. M., Rand D. G., & Pennycook G. (2021). Beyond “fake news”: Analytic thinking and the detection of false and hyperpartisan news headlines. Judgment and Decision Making, 16(2), 484-504. [78] Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 54-67. [79] Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30(4), 507-514. [80] Saltor J., Barberia I., & Rodríguez-Ferreiro J. (2023). Thinking disposition, thinking style, and susceptibility to causal illusion predict fake news discriminability. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 37(2), 360-368. [81] Sanchez, C., & Dunning, D. (2021). Cognitive and emotional correlates of belief in political misinformation: Who endorses partisan misbeliefs? Emotion, 21(5), 1091-1102. [82] Scherer L. D., McPhetres J., Pennycook G., Kempe A., Allen L. A., Knoepke C. E., Tate C. E., & Matlock D. D. (2021). Who is susceptible to online health misinformation? A test of four psychosocial hypotheses. Health Psychology, 40(4), 274-284. [83] Scheufele, D. A., & Krause, N. M. (2019). Science audiences, misinformation, and fake news. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(16), 7662-7669. [84] Schoenmueller V., Blanchard S. J., & Johar G. V. (2024). Who shares fake news? Uncovering insights from social media users'post histories. Journal of Marketing Research. Advanced online publication. [85] Simmons J. P., LeBoeuf R. A., & Nelson L. D. (2010). The effect of accuracy motivation on anchoring and adjustment: Do people adjust from provided anchors? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(6), 917-932. [86] Stanovich K. E., West R. F., & Toplak M. E. (2013). Myside bias, rational thinking, and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 259-264. [87] Stephen I. D., Burke D., & Sulikowski D. (2017). Tinbergen's “four questions” provides a formal framework for a more complete understanding of prosocial biases in favour of attractive people. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, Article e44. [88] Sun R. Y., Li C., Millet B., Ali K. I., & Petit J. (2022). Sharing news with online friends: A study of network homophily, network size, and news type. Telematics and Informatics, 67, Article 101763. [89] Talwar S., Dhir A., Kaur P., & Zafar N. (2019). Why do people share fake news? Associations between the dark side of social media use and fake news sharing behavior. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 51, 72-82. [90] Tangcharoensathien V., Calleja N., Nguyen T., Purnat T., D'Agostino M., Garcia-Saiso S., .. Briand S. (2020). Framework for managing the COVID-19 infodemic: Methods and results of an online, crowdsourced WHO technical consultation. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 22(6), Article e19659. [91] Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie, 20(4), 410-433. [92] van Bavel, J. J., & Pereira, A. (2018). The partisan brain: An identity-based model of political belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(3), 213-224. [93] van der Linden S., Panagopoulos C., Azevedo F., & Jost J. T. (2021). The paranoid style in American politics revisited: An ideological asymmetry in conspiratorial thinking. Political Psychology, 42(1), 23-51. [94] van Prooijen, J. W., & Douglas, K. M. (2018). Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(7), 897-908. [95] van Prooijen, J. W., & van Vugt, M. (2018). Conspiracy theories: Evolved functions and psychological mechanisms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(6), 770-788. [96] Verma R., Saldanha C., Ellis U., Sattar S., & Haase K. R. (2022). eHealth literacy among older adults living with cancer and their caregivers: A scoping review. Journal of Geriatric Oncology, 13(5), 555-562. [97] Vosoughi S., Roy D., & Aral S. (2018). The spread of true and false news online. Science, 359(6380), 1146-1151. [98] Walraven A., Brand-Gruwel S., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2010). Fostering transfer of websearchers'evaluation skills: A field test of two transfer theories. Computers in Human Behavior, 26(4), 716-728. [99] Wang R., He Y., Xu J., & Zhang H. Z. (2020). Fake news or bad news? Toward an emotion-driven cognitive dissonance model of misinformation diffusion. Asian Journal of Communication, 30(5), 317-342. [100] Weeks, B. E. (2015). Emotions, partisanship, and misperceptions: How anger and anxiety moderate the effect of partisan bias on susceptibility to political misinformation. Journal of Communication, 65(4), 699-719. [101] Wilck, A. M. (2021). Intense news: The role of emotion in the perception of (fake) news (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University at Albany, State University of New York. [102] Wineburg S., Breakstone J., McGrew S., Smith M. D., & Ortega T. (2022). Lateral reading on the open internet: A district-wide field study in high school government classes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114(5), 893-909. [103] Wischnewski, M., & Krämer, N. (2021). The role of emotions and identity-protection cognition when processing (mis)information. Technology, Mind, and Behavior, 2(1), 1-14. [104] World Economic Forum. (2024, January 10). Global risks report 2024. https://www.weforum.org/publications/global-risks-report-2024/ [105] Xu S. A., Shtulman A., & Young A. G. (2022). Can children detect fake news? Proceedings of the 44th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Toronto, ON. [106] Yelbuz B. E., Madan E., & Alper S. (2022). Reflective thinking predicts lower conspiracy beliefs: A meta-analysis. Judgment and Decision Making, 17(4), 720-744. [107] Yong J. C., Li N. P., & Kanazawa S. (2021). Not so much rational but rationalizing: Humans evolved as coherence-seeking, fiction-making animals. American Psychologist, 76(5), 781-793. |
[1] | . The influence of emotion on false memory and its application [J]. Psychological Science, 2011, 34(4): 813-818. |
Viewed | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Full text 220
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Abstract 177
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||