Bruner, J. S., & Potter, M. C. (1964). Interference in visual recognition. Science, 144, 424-425.
Browm, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: evidence from masked priming. J Cogn Neurosci, 5, 34–44.
Statisics and analysis of Mandarin Chinese vocabulary(in Chinese). (1986). Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Press.
[现代汉语频率词典. (1986). 北京:北京语言学院出版社.]
Carpenter, G. A., & Grossberg, S. (1986). Adaptive resonance theory: Stable self- organization of neural recognition codes in response to arbitrary lists of input patterns. In Program of the Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum, 45–62.
Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., & Yang, C.H., et al. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 137–146.
Frishkoff, G. A. (2007). Hemispheric differences in strong versus weak semantic priming: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Lang, 100, 23–43。
Galloway, D. W. (1946). An experimental investigation of structural lag in perception [Abstract]. American Psychologist,1, 450.
Gibson, J. M., & Watkins, M. J. (1991). An Auditory Cue-Depreciation Effect. The American Journal of Psychology, 104 (3), 439.
Grossberg, S. (1976). Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding, II: Feedback, expectation, olfaction, and illusion. Biological Cybernetics, 23, 187–202
Goldstein, A., Spencer, K. M. & Donchin, E. (2002). The influence of stimulus deviance and novelty on the P300 and Novelty P3. Psychophysiology, 39(6), 781–790.
He, H. D., & Jiao, S. L. (1994). Part-set cuing effect in visual identification. Acta Psychologica Sinica,26(3),264–271.
[何海东, 焦书兰. (1994). 图形和汉字视觉任务中的部分线索效应的研究. 心理学报, 26 (3), 264–271.]
Hillyard, S. A., Squires, K. C., & Bauer, J. W., et al. (1971). Evoked potential correlates of auditory signal detection. Science,172, 1357–1360.
J. Bruno Debruille. (1998). knowledge inhibition and N400: A study with words that look like common words. Brain snd Language, 62, 202–220.
Jonides J. Voluntary vs. automatic control over the mind's eye's movement. In: JB Long, A.D.Baddeley (Eds), Attention and performance IX. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1981,187–203.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207 (4427), 203–205.
Kiefer, M., & Spitzer, M. (2000). Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activations. NeuroReport, 11, 2401–2407.
Luo, C. H. R., & Snodgrass, J. G. (1994). Competitive Activation Model of Perceptual Interference in picture and word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20 (1) ,50–60.
Li, F. F. , & Wang, Q. H. (2007). The role of study, frequency and structural style in the perception interference effect on Chinese characters (in Chinese). Psychological Science, 30(3), 547–551.
[李菲菲, 王权红. (2007). 学习,频率和结构方式对汉字知觉线索毁损效应的影响. 心理科学, 30 (3), 547–551.]
Li, F. F. , & Wang, Q. H. (2008). How starting point of blurred cues and viewing time influence the perception interference effect on Chinese characters (in Chinese). Psychological Science, 31(5), 1176–1180.
[李菲菲, 王权红. (2008). 线索起点模糊水平和注视时间对汉字知觉干扰效应的影响. 心理科学, 31 (5), 1176–1180.]
McCarthy, G., & Nobre, A. (1993). Modulation of semantic processing by spatial selective attention. Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol, 88, 210–219.
McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 168–180.
Peynircioglu, Z. F. (1990). Amount of priming and the cue depreciation effect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 393–394.
Peynircioglu, Z. F., & Watkins, M. C. (1986). Cue depreciation: When word fragment completion is undermined by prior exposure of lesser fragments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 426–431.
Pesciarelli, F., Kutas, M., & Dell’Acqua, R., et al. (2007). Semantic and repetition priming within the attentional blink: An event-related brain potential (ERP) investigation study. Biol Psychol, 76, 21–30.
Snodgrass, J. G., & Hirshman, E. (1991). Theoretical explorations of the Bruner-Potter (1964) interference effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 273–293.
Schulkind, M. D. (2002). Feature modulation search: A novel memory search model that extends the perceptual interference effect to musical stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory and Cognition, 28, 346–352.
Schulkind, M. D. (2004). Conceptual and perceptual information both influence melody identification. Memory & Cognition, 32, 841–851.
Thapar, A., & Greene, R. L. (1995). A st rategic account of the cue-depreciation effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49 (4), 513–519.
Thorpe, S., Fize, D., & Marlot, C. (1996). Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature, 381, 520–522.
Van Petten.C., & Kutas, M. (1991). Electrophysiological evidence for the flexibility of lexical processing. In G. Simpson (Ed.), Word and sentence, (pp.129-174). Amsterdam: North Holland Press.
Wang, Q. H., & Reinitz, M .T. (2001). Effects of the number and st rength of competing solutions on the Perceptual Interference Effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance, 27 (1), 22–31.
Wang, Q. H., Li, F. F., & He, M. (2007). Comparisons of activation Levels of object picture and chinese words, and of their perceptual interence effects (in Chinese). Psychological Science, 31(5), 1176–1180.
[王权红, 李菲菲, 何敏. (2007). 图片和汉字激活水平与知觉干扰效应的比较. 心理科学, 30 (2) , 332–336.]
Zhang, Y. H., & Guo, C.Y. (2008). An ERP study on the relationship between Perceptual and semantic representation (in Chinese). Chinese Science Bulletin, 53(24), 3086–3095.
[张运红, 郭春彦. (2008). 知觉和语义表征关系ERP研究. 科学通报,53(24), 3086–3095.]
|