Journal of Psychological Science ›› 2024, Vol. 47 ›› Issue (2): 258-266.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20240201
• General Psychology, Experimental Psychology & Ergonomics • Next Articles
Si Shuangqing, Zhou Sihong, Yuan Jiajin, Yang Qian
Online:
2024-03-20
Published:
2024-02-29
司双庆, 周思宏, 袁加锦, 杨倩**
通讯作者:
**杨倩,E-mail: qianyang_psy@163.com
基金资助:
Si Shuangqing, Zhou Sihong, Yuan Jiajin, Yang Qian. Influencing Factors in the Allocation of Cognitive Control: Rewards and Costs[J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(2): 258-266.
司双庆, 周思宏, 袁加锦, 杨倩. 认知控制过程的影响因素:回报与代价*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(2): 258-266.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://jps.ecnu.edu.cn/EN/10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20240201
[1] 曹思琪, 汤晨晨, 伍海燕, 刘勋. (2022). 价值计算决定何时与如何努力. 心理科学进展, 30(4), 877-887. [2] 杨倩. (2022). 负性情绪在冲突适应中的作用机制: 分离与整合视角. 心理科学进展, 30(8), 1844-1855. [3] 易伟, 梅淑婷, 郑亚. (2019). 努力: 成本还是奖赏? 心理科学进展, 27(8), 1439-1450. [4] 张孟可, 李晴, 尹首航, 陈安涛. (2021). 冲突水平的变化诱发冲突适应. 心理学报, 53(2), 128-138. [5] Aarts E., Wallace D. L., Dang L. C., Jagust W. J., Cools R., & D’Esposito M. (2014). Dopamine and the cognitive downside of a promised bonus. Psychological Science, 25(4), 1003-1009. [6] André N., Audiffren M., & Baumeister R. F. (2019). An integrative model of effortful control. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13, Article 79. [7] Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610-615. [8] Bahlmann J., Aarts E., & D’Esposito M. (2015). Influence of motivation on control hierarchy in the human frontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 3207-3217. [9] Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration: Animals spend much of their time seeking stimuli whose significance raises problems for psychology. Science, 153(3731), 25-33. [10] Boehler C. N., Hopf J. M., Stoppel C. M., & Krebs R. M. (2012). Motivating inhibition - reward prospect speeds up response cancellation. Cognition, 125(3), 498-503. [11] Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 83-113. [12] Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 356-366. [13] Botvinick M. M., Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Carter C. S., & Cohen J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652. [14] Bustamante L., Lieder F., Musslick S., Shenhav A., & Cohen J. (2021). Learning to overexert cognitive control in a Stroop task. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 21(3), 453-471. [15] Bustamante, L. A. (2022). Quantifying individual differences in the cost of cognitive and physical effort in humans (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Princeton University. [16] Bialaszek, W., & Winkielman, P. (2023). Modeling the Effort Paradox: Effort can have positive and negative impact on outcome value within an individual, across individuals, and across choice contexts (preprint) [17] Cacioppo J. T., Petty R. E., Feinstein J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197-253. [18] Camerer, C. F., & Hogarth, R. M. (1999). The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19(1), 7-42. [19] Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319-333. [20] Charpentier C. J., Bromberg-Martin E. S., & Sharot T. (2018). Valuation of knowledge and ignorance in mesolimbic reward circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(31), E7255-E7264. [21] Chew B., Blain B., Dolan R. J., & Rutledge R. B. (2021). A neurocomputational model for intrinsic reward. The Journal of Neuroscience, 41(43), 8963-8971. [22] Chiew, K. S. (2021). Revisiting positive affect and reward influences on cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 27-33. [23] Cromwell H. C., Abe N., Barrett K. C., Caldwell-Harris C., Gendolla G. H. E., Koncz R., & Sachdev P. S. (2020). Mapping the interconnected neural systems underlying motivation and emotion: A key step toward understanding the human affectome. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 113, 204-226. [24] Cubillo A., Makwana A. B., & Hare T. A. (2019). Differential modulation of cognitive control networks by monetary reward and punishment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(3), 305-317. [25] Davis C., Levitan R. D., Kaplan A. S., Carter J., Reid C., Curtis C., & Kennedy J. L. (2008). Reward sensitivity and the D2 dopamine receptor gene: A case-control study of binge eating disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 32(3), 620-628. [26] Devine S., Vassena E., & Otto A. R. (2023). More than a feeling: Physiological measures of affect index the integration of effort costs and rewards during anticipatory effort evaluation. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 23(4), 1129-1140. [27] Dignath D., Eder A. B., Steinhauser M., & Kiesel A. (2020). Conflict monitoring and the affective-signaling hypothesis—An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27(2), 193-216. [28] Dixon, M. L., & Christoff, K. (2012). The decision to engage cognitive control is driven by expected reward-value: Neural and behavioral evidence. PLoS ONE, 7(12), Article e51637. [29] Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition, 78(2), 94-98. [30] Engelmann J. B., Damaraju E., Padmala S., & Pessoa L. (2009). Combined effects of attention and motivation on visual task performance: Transient and sustained motivational effects. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, Article 4. [31] Fleischhauer M., Enge S., Brocke B., Ullrich J., Strobel A., & Strobel A. (2010). Same or different? Clarifying the relationship of need for cognition to personality and intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(1), 82-96. [32] Fröber, K., & Dreisbach, G. (2016). How sequential changes in reward magnitude modulate cognitive flexibility: Evidence from voluntary task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(2), 285-295. [33] Froböse M. I., Westbrook A., Bloemendaal M., Aarts E., & Cools R. (2020). Catecholaminergic modulation of the cost of cognitive control in healthy older adults. PLoS ONE, 15(2), Article e0229294. [34] Grahek I., Leng X., Musslick S., & Shenhav A. (2023). The cost of adjusting cognitive control: A dynamical systems approach. Paper presented at the 2023 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience. [35] Grahek I., Musslick S., & Shenhav A. (2020). A computational perspective on the roles of affect in cognitive control. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 151, 25-34. [36] Hassall C. D., Hunt L. T., & Holroyd C. B. (2022). Task-level value affects trial-level reward processing. NeuroImage, 260, Article 119456. [37] Huskey R., Craighead B., Miller M. B., & Weber R. (2018). Does intrinsic reward motivate cognitive control? A naturalistic-fMRI study based on the synchronization theory of flow. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(5), 902-924. [38] Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450-463. [39] Inzlicht M., Shenhav A., & Olivola C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337-349. [40] Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2018). Mental labour. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(12), 899-908. [41] Kool W., McGuire J. T., Rosen Z. B., & Botvinick M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 665-682. [42] Kool W., Shenhav A., & Botvinick M. M. (2017). Cognitive control as cost-benefit decision making. In T. Egner (Ed.), The Wiley handbook of cognitive control (pp. 167-189). Wiley Blackwell. [43] Kruglanski A. W., Fishbach A., Woolley K., Bélanger J. J., Chernikova M., Molinario E., & Pierro A. (2018). A structural model of intrinsic motivation: On the psychology of means-ends fusion. Psychological Review, 125(2), 165-182. [44] Kurzban, R. (2016). The sense of effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 67-70. [45] Kurzban R., Duckworth A., Kable J. W., & Myers J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661-679. [46] Leng X. M., Yee D., Ritz H., & Shenhav A. (2021). Dissociable influences of reward and punishment on adaptive cognitive control. PLoS Computational Biology, 17(12), Article e1009737. [47] Li Q., Hu N., Li Y. L., Long Q. S., Gu Y., Tang Y. C., & Chen A. T. (2021). Error-induced adaptability: Behavioral and neural dynamics of response-stimulus interval modulations on posterror slowing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(5), 851-863. [48] Lieder F., Shenhav A., Musslick S., & Griffiths T. L. (2018). Rational metareasoning and the plasticity of cognitive control. PLoS Computational Biology, 14(4), Article e1006043. [49] Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 99-112. [50] Marcowski P., Winkielman P., & Białaszek W. (2023). Effort can have positive and negative impact on outcome value within an individual, across individuals, and across choice contexts. PsyArXiv (preprint). [51] Masís J. A., Musslick S., & Cohen J. D. (2021). The value of learning and cognitive control allocation. Paper presented at the 43rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society: Animal Minds, Virtual, Online, Austria. [52] Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167-202. [53] Morris L. S., Grehl M. M., Rutter S. B., Mehta M., & Westwater M. L. (2022). On what motivates us: A detailed review of intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivation. Psychological Medicine, 52(10), 1801-1816. [54] Notebaert, W., & Braem, S. (2015). Parsing the effects of reward on cognitive control. In T. S. Braver (Ed.), Motivation and cognitive control (pp. 105-122). Routledge. [55] Otto A. R., Braem S., Silvetti M., & Vassena E. (2022). Is the juice worth the squeeze? Learning the marginal value of mental effort over time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(10), 2324-2341. [56] Otto, A. R., & Vassena, E. (2021). It' s all relative: Reward-induced cognitive control modulation depends on context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(2), 306-313. [57] Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2011). Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3419-3432. [58] Prével A., Hoofs V., & Krebs R. M. (2021). Effect of non-instructed instrumental contingency of monetary reward and positive affect in a cognitive control task. Royal Society Open Science, 8(8), Article 202002. [59] Sandra, D. A., & Otto, A. R. (2018). Cognitive capacity limitations and Need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure. Cognition, 172, 101-106. [60] Saunders B., Lin H., Milyavskaya M., & Inzlicht M. (2017). The emotive nature of conflict monitoring in the medial prefrontal cortex. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 119, 31-40. [61] Sayalı, C., & Badre, D. (2019). Neural systems of cognitive demand avoidance. Neuropsychologia, 123, 41-54. [62] Sayalı, C., & Badre, D. (2021). Neural systems underlying the learning of cognitive effort costs. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 21(4), 698-716. [63] Schouppe N., Braem S., De Houwer J., Silvetti M., Verguts T., Ridderinkhof K. R., & Notebaert W. (2015). No pain, no gain: The affective valence of congruency conditions changes following a successful response. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(1), 251-261. [64] Schouppe N., Demanet J., Boehler C. N., Ridderinkhof K. R., & Notebaert W. (2014). The role of the striatum in effort-based decision-making in the absence of reward. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(6), 2148-2154. [65] Sharp P. B., Do K. T., Lindquist K. A., Prinstein M. J., & Telzer E. H. (2022). Cognitive control deployment is flexibly modulated by social value in early adolescence. Developmental Science, 25(1), Article e13140. [66] Shenhav A., Botvinick M. M., & Cohen J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217-240. [67] Shenhav A., Musslick S., Lieder F., Kool W., Griffiths T. L., Cohen J. D., & Botvinick M. M. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40, 99-124. [68] Stürmer, B. (2011). Reward and punishment effects on error processing and conflict control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 335. [69] van Steenbergen H., Band G. P. H., & Hommel B. (2012). Reward valence modulates conflict-driven attentional adaptation: Electrophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology, 90(3), 234-241. [70] Vermeylen L., Braem S., Notebaert W., & Ruitenberg, M. F. L. (2022). The subjective evaluation of task switch cues is related to voluntary task switching. Cognition, 224, Article 105063. [71] Wang, K. S., & Delgado, M. R. (2019). Corticostriatal circuits encode the subjective value of perceived control. Cerebral Cortex, 29(12), 5049-5060. [72] Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(2), 395-415. [73] Westbrook A., Frank M. J., & Cools R. (2021). A mosaic of cost-benefit control over cortico-striatal circuitry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(8), 710-721. [74] Westbrook A., Kester D., & Braver T. S. (2013). What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PLoS ONE, 8(7), Article e68210. [75] Westbrook A., Van Den Bosch R., Määttä J. I., Hofmans L., Papadopetraki D., Cools R., & Frank M. J. (2020). Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work. Science, 367(6484), 1362-1366. [76] Yee D. M., Krug M. K., Allen A. Z., & Braver T. S. (2016). Humans integrate monetary and liquid incentives to motivate cognitive task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 2037. [77] Zhang M., Palmer C. V., Pratt S. R., McNeil M. R., & Siegle G. J. (2022). Need for cognition is associated with the interaction of reward and task-load on effort: A verification and extension study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 180, 60-67. |
[1] | Li Ying, Li Lin, Pan Jiabing, Lu Xiaoxiao, Wang Yue. The Effect of Executive Control Training on Language Control of Chinese-English Bilinguals with Different Working Memory Updating Ability [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(5): 1088-1095. |
[2] | Xiao Jingjing, Zhang Lijuan, Chen Dengshui, Luo Shuang, Zhang Jinkun. Task Switching Affects Word Pairs Memory Performance: The Compensatory Effects of Retrieval Practice [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(2): 308-315. |
[3] | Fan Wei, Yang Ying, Dong Yanqiu, Zhang Wenjie. The Effects of Competitive Situations and Reward Motivation on Self-Deception: Evidence From Eye Movements [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(2): 411-423. |
[4] | Wang Zhijing, Li Fuhong. Pupillary Response and Brain Mechanisms of Cognitive Control [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(1): 2-10. |
[5] | Wang Guanhua, Lu Jiachen, Li Hong, Lei Yi. Inducing High Self-Efficacy Improves Cognitive Control after Self Depletion [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(6): 1282-1289. |
[6] | Zhou Xing, Hao Shuang, Zhao Lili, He Weiqi. Effects of Reward Learning on Attention Capture of Task-Irrelevant Emotional Faces [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(6): 1298-1304. |
[7] | Yu Tengxu, Hu Jinsheng. A Review of Adaptive and Maladaptive Risk Taking Behavior among Adolescents [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(6): 1440-1446. |
[8] | Ba Junxiu, Wang Lijun. Sensitivity of Depression-Inclined Individuals to Social and Monetary Rewards: The Role of Reward Probability [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(5): 1220-1227. |
[9] | Gao Bin, Jia Yingfang, Jiang Yunpeng, Wu Jie. The Influence of Reward Motivation on Automatic Reaction Inhibition [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(4): 795-801. |
[10] | Zhang Huan, Zuo Tianran, Liu Yibei, Liu Xiping, Yang Haibo. The Cognitive Mechanism of Socially Shared Retrieval-Induced Forgetting: Inhibition or Non-Inhibition? [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(4): 802-808. |
[11] | Yue Yang, Jiang Yingjie, Long Yiting, Wang Kaiyu. Effect of Unsigned Reward Prediction Error on Mnemonic Discrimination: Evidence from Behavioral and Eye-Tracking [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(4): 817-824. |
[12] | Liu Zhengjie, Zhou Xiaojuan, Li Qingxuan, Zeng Ziyang, Li Hong, Zhang Zhongming. The Prediction of Reward Positive and Late Positive Potential to Depressive Tendency: An ERP Study [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(4): 980-990. |
[13] | Yu Rui, Tao Yun, Zhu Xiaoyuan, Tian Tao. Do the bilinguals have better level of conflict monitoring? [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(3): 522-529. |
[14] | Shen Chengchun , Hu bohua , He Qinghua . The effect of stress on decision-making under ambiguity: cognitive and neural mechanisms [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(2): 500-508. |
[15] | . The Prioritization of Self-associated Stimuli on Task Switching and Target Shifting [J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2022, 45(5): 1053-1060. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||