Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Chien-Ming, Y., Nagata, M. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process, Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 137-146. Gilbert, C. D., Sigman, M. (2007). Brain states: Top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron, 54, 677-696. Kahan, T. A. (2000). Negative priming from masked words: Retrospective prime clarification or center-surround inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1392-1410. Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 27-39. Kiefer, M. (2006). Top-down Modulation automatischer Prozesse durch Aufgabeneinstellungen [Top down modulation of automatic processes by task sets]. Paper presented at the Teap, Meinz, Germany. Kiefer, M., Brendel, D. (2006). Attentional modulation of unconscious ‘automatic’ processes: Evidence from event-related potentials in a masked priming paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 184-198. Luck, S. J., Vogel, E. K., Shapiro, K.L. (1996). Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature, 383, 616–618. Mari-Beffa, P., Valdes, B., Cullen, D. J. D. et al. (2005). ERP analyses of task effects on semantic processing from words. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 293-305. Miller, B. T., D’ Esposito, M. (2005). Searching for “the Top” in top-down control. Neuron, 48, 535-538. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55-85). HillSDale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., Henninghausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38, 165-174. Schneider, W., Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1-66. Smith, J. E. K. (1982). Simple algorithms for M-alternative forced-choice calculations. Perception and Psychophysics, 31(1), 95-96. Sui, J., Zhu, Y., Han, S. H. (2006). Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 17(4), 423-427. Taft, M., Zhu, X. P. (1999). Positional Specificity of Radicals in Chinese Character Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 498-519. Verleger, R., Ja?kowski, P., Aydemir, A., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative differences between conscious and non-conscious processing? On negative and positive priming effects induced by masked arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 494-515. Wager, T. D., Jonides, J., Reading, S. (2004). Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 22, 1679-1693. Liu, W. (2004). An experiment study of semantic transparency in the processing of compounds. Unpublished Doctorial Dissertation. Beijing Language and Culture University. [刘伟. (2004). 语义透明度对留学生双音节合成词词汇通达的影响. 博士学位论文. 北京语言大学.] |