[1] 曹漱芹, 方俊明. (2010). 自闭症儿童汉语词汇语义加工和图片语义加工的实验研究. 中国特殊教育, 10, 57-62. [2] 黄静, 梁丹丹. (2013). 自闭症个体语义加工障碍综述. 当代语言学, 15(3), 349-357. [3] 舒华, 程元善, 张厚粲. (1989). 235个图形的命名一致性、熟悉性、表象一致性和视觉复杂性评定. 心理学报, 21(4), 389-396. [4] 张积家, 宋宜琪. (2012). 盲人的时间水平方向隐喻的通道特异性. 心理学报, 44(1), 40-50. [5] American Psychiatric Association.(2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5TM. American Psychiatric Publishing. [6] Bochynska A., Coventry K. R., Vulchanov V., & Vulchanova M. (2020). Tell me where it is: Selective difficulties in spatial language on the autism spectrum. Autism, 24(7), 1740-1757. [7] Boroditsky L., Fuhrman O., & McCormick K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118(1), 123-129. [8] Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351-367. [9] Eskes G. A., Bryson S. E., & McCormick T. A. (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 61-73. [10] Estes Z., Verges M., & Barsalou L. W. (2008). Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects' typical location. Psychological Science, 19(2), 93-97. [11] Fodor J. A.(1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press. [12] Gozli D. G., Pratt J., Martin K. Z., & Chasteen A. L. (2016). Implied spatial meaning and visuospatial bias: Conceptual processing influences processing of visual targets and distractors. PLoS ONE, 11(3), Article e0150928. [13] Just M. A., Keller T. A., Malave V. L., Kana R. K., & Varma S. (2012). Autism as a neural systems disorder: A theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1292-1313. [14] Knaus T. A., Burns C., Kamps J., & Foundas A. L. (2017). Atypical activation of action-semantic network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 117, 57-64. [15] Krug D. A., Arick J., & Almond P. (1980). Behavior checklist for identifying severely handicapped individuals with high levels of autistic behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21(3), 221-229. [16] Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96-104. [17] Mari M., Castiello U., Marks D., Marraffa C., & Prior M. (2003). The reach-to-grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 393-403. [18] Mooney L. N., Nordahl C. W., Solomon M., & Ghetti S. (2020). Children with ASD show impaired item-space recollection, but preserved item-color recollection. Autism Research, 13(11), 1985-1997. [19] Moseley R. L., Mohr B., Lombardo M. V., Baron-Cohen S., Hauk O., & Pulvermu?ller F. (2013). Brain and behavioral correlates of action semantic deficits in autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 725. [20] Moseley, R. L., & Pulvermüller, F. (2018). What can autism teach us about the role of sensorimotor systems in higher cognition? New clues from studies on language, action semantics, and abstract emotional concept processing. Cortex, 100, 149-190. [21] Moseley R. L., Shtyrov Y., Mohr B., Lombardo M. V., Baron-Cohen S., & Pulvermüller F. (2015). Lost for emotion words: What motor and limbic brain activity reveals about autism and semantic theory. NeuroImage, 104, 413-422. [22] Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383-399. [23] Peleg O., Ozer R., Norman T., & Segal O. (2018). Perceptual simulations during sentence comprehension: A comparison between typical adolescents and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 45, 36-44. [24] Rommers J., Meyer A. S., & Huettig F. (2013). Object shape and orientation do not routinely influence performance during language processing. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2218-2225. [25] Smith L. B.,& Samuelson, L. K. (2010). Objects in space and mind: From reaching to words In K S Mix, L B Smith, & M Gasser (Eds), The spatial foundations of language and cognition (pp 188-207) Oxford University Press From reaching to words. In K. S. Mix, L. B. Smith, & M. Gasser (Eds.), The spatial foundations of language and cognition (pp. 188-207). Oxford University Press. [26] South M., Ozonoff S., & McMahon W. M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 145-158. [27] Sutera S., Pandey J., Esser E. L., Rosenthal M. A., Wilson L. B., Barton M., & Fein D. (2007). Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 98-107. [28] Vogt A., Kaup B., & Dudschig C. (2019). When words are upside down: Language-space associations in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 186, 142-158. [29] Yi L., Fan Y. B., Zhao J., Huang D., Li Y. Y., & Zou X. B. (2013). Atypical understanding of mental terms in Chinese-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(11), 1411-1417. [30] Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semantic relatedness judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(4), 954-958. |