[1] 何清华, 薛贵, 陈春辉, 董奇, 陈传升. (2014). 遗传因素在风险决策加工中的作用. 心理科学进展, 22(2), 191-204. [2] 黄仁辉, 曾晓青, 李文虎. (2014). 不确定性容忍度、损益概率及延迟风险对延迟选择的影响. 心理学探新, 24(3), 230-235. [3] 李晓明, 蒋松源. (2019). 权力对延迟选择的影响. 心理科学进展, 27(3), 71-76. [4] 李晓明, 谢佳. (2012). 偶然情绪对延迟选择的影响机制. 心理学报, 44(12), 83-92. [5] 梁竹苑, 许燕, 蒋奖. (2007). 决策中个体差异研究现状述评. 心理科学进展, 15(4), 689-694. [6] 王怀勇, 刘永芳. (2013). 调节定向与延迟风险对决策偏好的影响. 心理研究, 6(2), 56-63. [7] 王怀勇, 刘永芳. (2014). 决策过程中的调节匹配效应及其机制. 心理科学, 37(1), 182-189. [8] 周蕾, 李纾, 许燕, 梁竹苑. (2014). 决策风格的理论发展及建构:基于信息加工视角. 心理科学进展, 22(1), 112-121. [9] 朱冬青, 谢晓非. (2013). 最优化与满意型决策风格孰优孰劣? 心理科学进展, 21(2), 309-316. [10] Anderson, C. J. (2003). The psychology of doing nothing: Forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 139-167. [11] Appelt K. C., Milch K. F., Handgraaf M., & Weber E. U. (2011). The decision making individual differences inventory and guidelines for the study of individual differences in judgment and decision-making research. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(3), 252-262. [12] Bahník, Š. (2019). Disfluent, but fast: inverted-u shaped effect of fluency on decision times. Experimental Psychology, 66(1), 1-9. [13] Berens, S., & Funke, J. (2020). A vignette study of option refusal and decision deferral as two forms of decision avoidance: Situational and personal predictors. PLoS ONE, 15(10), e0241182. [14] Chatterjee, S., & Heath, T. B. (1996). Conflict and loss aversion in multiattribute choice: The effects of trade-off size and reference dependence on decision difficulty. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67(2), 144-155. [15] Cheek, N. N., & Goebel, J. (2020). What does it mean to maximize? "Decision difficulty", indecisiveness, and the jingle-jangle fallacies in the measurement of maximizing. Judgment and Decision Making, 15(1), 7-24. [16] Chen Y., Ma X., & Pethtel O. (2011). Age differences in trade-off decisions: Older adults prefer choice deferral. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 269-273. [17] Chernev A., Böckenholt U., & Goodman J. (2010). Commentary on Scheibehenne, Greifeneder, and Todd choice overload: Is there anything to it? Journal of Consumer Research, 37(3), 426-428. [18] Chernev A., Böckenholt U., & Goodman J. (2015). Choice overload: A conceptual review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 333-358. [19] Chowdhury T. G., Ratneshwar S., & Mohanty P. (2009). The time-harried shopper: Exploring the differences between maximizers and satisficers. Marketing Letters, 20(2), 155-167. [20] Dalal D. K., Diab D. L., Zhu X. Y., & Hwang T. (2015). Understanding the construct of maximizing tendency: A theoretical and empirical evaluation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28, 437-450. [21] Dar-Nimrod I., Rawn C. D., Lehman D. R., & Schwartz B. (2009). The maximization paradox: The costs of seeking alternatives. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5), 631-635. [22] Deck, C., & Jahedi, S. (2015). The effect of cognitive load on economic decision making: A survey and new experiments. European Economic Review, 78, 97-119. [23] Dhar, R. (1996). The effect of decision strategy on deciding to defer choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9(4), 265-281. [24] Dhar, R. (1997). Consumer preference for a no-choice option. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(2), 215-231. [25] Dhar, R., & Nowlis, S. M. (1999). The effect of time pressure on consumer choice deferral. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(4), 369-384. [26] Diab D. L., Gillespie M. A., & Highhouse S. (2008). Are maximizers really unhappy? The measurement of maximizing tendency. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(5), 364-370. [27] Drolet, A., & Luce, M. F. (2004). The rationalizing effects of cognitive load on emotion-based trade-off avoidance. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 63-77. [28] Etkin J., Ghosh A. P., Dahl D., & Laboo A. (2018). When being in a positive mood increases choice deferral. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(1), 208-225. [29] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavioral Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [30] Garg N., Inman J. J., & Mittal V. (2017). Emotion effects on choice deferral: The moderating role of outcome and process accountability European Journal of Marketing, 51(9), 1631-1649. [31] Gourville, J. T., & Soman, D. (2005). Overchoice and assortment type: When and why variety backfires. Marketing Science, 24(3), 382-395. [32] Inbar Y., Botti S., & Hanko K. (2011). Decision speed and choice regret: When haste feels like waste. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 533-540. [33] Iyengar S. S., Wells R. E., & Schwartz B. (2006). Doing better but feeling worse: Looking for the “best” job undermines satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17(2), 143-150. [34] Kim, K. & Miller, E. G. (2017). Vulnerable maximizers: The role of decision difficulty. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(5), 516-526. [35] Krijnen J. M. T., Zeelenberg M., & Breugelmans S. M. (2015). Decision importance as a cue for deferral. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(5), 407-415. [36] Larasati, A., & Yeh, M. Y. (2016). Does more attractive choice always decrease choice deferral? The moderating effect of ideal point. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 54, 43-51. [37] Leach, J. K., & Patall, E. A. (2013). Maximizing and counterfactual thinking in academic major decision making. Journal of Career Assessment, 21(3), 414-429. [38] Lohse, G. L., & Johnson, E. J. (1996). A comparison of two process tracing methods for choice tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68(1), 28-43. [39] Luce, M. F. (1998). Choosing to avoid: Coping with negatively emotion-laden consumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 409-433. [40] Luce M. F., Payne J. W., & Bettman J. R. (1999). Emotional trade-off difficulty and choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 36(2), 143-159. [41] Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (1998). Person-context interaction theories. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 685-759). John Wiley & Sons Inc. [42] Mao, W. (2016). When one desires too much of a good thing: The compromise effect under maximizing tendencies. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(1), 66-80. [43] Misuraca R., Faraci P., Gangemi A., Carmeci F. A., & Miceli S. (2015). The decision making tendency inventory: A new measure to assess maximizing, satisficing, and minimizing. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 111-116. [44] Misuraca, R., & Teuscher, U. (2013). Time flies when you maximize — maximizers and satisficers perceive time differently when making decisions. Acta Psychologica, 143(2), 176-180. [45] Noguchi, T., & Hills, T. T. (2016). Description-experience gap in choice deferral. Decision, 3(1), 54-61. [46] Novemsky N., Dhar R., Schwarz N., & Simonson, I.(20007). Preference fluency in choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(3), 347-356. [47] Owen H. E., Halberstadt J., Carr E. W., & Winkielman P. (2016). Johnny depp, reconsidered: How category-relative processing fluency determines the appeal of gender ambiguity. PLoS ONE, 11(2), e0146328. [48] Parker A. M., Bruin W., & Fischhoff B. (2007). Maximizers versus satisficers: Decision-making styles, competence, and outcomes. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(6), 342-350. [49] Pethtel, O. L., & Chen, Y. (2013). Age differences in choice deferrals as functions of interattribute conflict and decision domain. Educational Gerontology, 39(10), 772-782. [50] Pilli, L. E., & Mazzon, J. A. (2016). Information overload, choice deferral, and moderating role of need for cognition: Empirical evidence. Revista de Administração, 51(1), 36-55. [51] Polman, E. (2010). Why are maximizers less happy than satisficers? Because they maximize positive and negative outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(2), 179-190. [52] Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2005). Indecisiveness and the interpretation of ambiguous situations. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1285-1291. [53] Reed D. D., Reed F. D. D., Chok J., & Brozyna G. A. (2011). The "tyranny of choice": Choice overload as a possible instance of effort discounting. The Psychological Record, 61(4), 547-560. [54] Richardson C. M., Ye H. J., Ege E., Suh H., & Rice K. G. (2014). Refining the measurement of maximization: Gender invariance and relation to psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 229-234. [55] Rieskamp J.,& Hoffrage, U. (1999). When do people use simple heuristics and how can we tell. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, & the ABC Research Group (eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 141-167). Oxford University Press. [56] Rim, H. B. (2012). Maximizing, satisficing and their impacts on decision-making behaviors. The Ohio State University. [57] Rogge, N. (2017). Maximizing, choice freedom, and duration judgments in choice making. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 16(6), e125-e138. [58] Saltsman T. L., Seery M. D., Ward D. E., Radsvick T. M., Panlilio Z. A., Lamarche V. M., & Kondrak C. L. (2020). Facing the facets: No association between dispositional mindfulness facets and positive momentary stress responses during active stressors. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1-14. [59] Schwartz B., Ward A., Monterosso J., Lyubomirsky S., White K., & Lehman D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1178-1197. [60] Shiloh S., Koren S., & Dan Z. (2001). Individual differences in compensatory decision-making style and need for closure as correlates of subjective decision complexity and difficulty. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 699-710. [61] Spunt R. P., Rassin E., & Epstein L. M. (2009). Aversive and avoidant indecisiveness: Roles for regret proneness, maximization, and bias sensitivities. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(4), 256-261. [62] Unkelbach, C., & Rom, S. C. (2017). A referential theory of the repetition-induced truth effect. Cognition, 160, 110-126. [63] Weinhardt J. M., Morse B. J., Chimeli J., & Fisher J. (2012). An item response theory and factor analytic examination of two prominent maximizing tendency scales. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(5), 644-658. [64] Yang, M. L., & Chiou, W. B. (2010). Looking online for the best romantic partner reduces decision quality: The moderating role of choice-making strategies. Cyberpsychology Behavior and Social Networking, 13(2), 207-210. |