[1] 黄希庭, 郭秀艳, 聂晶. (2003). 认知加工中时间与非时间信息的相互关系. 心理科学, 26(5), 770-774. [2] 梅慧娴, 周治金, 陈其才. (2013). 听觉中脑声定位的机制. 心理科学进展, 21(6), 1028-1033. [3] 王碧瑶, 陈晨, 胡晓斐, 王迪, 李宝林. (2024). 视觉时距知觉序列依赖效应的空间迁移性. 心理学报, 56(4), 394-411. [4] Akaike, H. (2011). International encyclopedia of statistical science. Springer. [5] Alais, D., & Burr, D. (2004). The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Current Biology, 14(3), 257-262. [6] Becker, M. W., & Rasmussen, I. P. (2007). The rhythm aftereffect: Support for time sensitive neurons with broad overlapping tuning curves. Brain and Cognition, 64(3), 274-281. [7] Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433-436. [8] Brimijoin, W. O., & O'Neill, W. E. (2010). Patterned tone sequences reveal non-linear interactions in auditory spectrotemporal receptive fields in the inferior colliculus. Hearing Research, 267(1-2), 96-110. [9] Bruno A., Ayhan I., & Johnston A. (2010). Retinotopic adaptation-based visual duration compression. Journal of Vision, 10(10), Article 30. [10] Cicchini G. M., Benedetto A., & Burr D. C. (2021). Perceptual history propagates down to early levels of sensory analysis. Current Biology, 31(6), 1245-1250.e2. [11] Collins, T. (2019). The perceptual continuity field is retinotopic. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 18841. [12] Collins, T. (2022). Serial dependence occurs at the level of both features and integrated object representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(8), 1821-1832. [13] Fang F., Murray S. O., Kersten D., & He S. (2005). Orientation-tuned fMRI adaptation in human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94(6), 4188-4195. [14] Feigin H., Baror S., Bar M., & Zaidel A. (2021). Perceptual decisions are biased toward relevant prior choices. Scientific Reports, 11(1), Article 648. [15] Fischer C., Czoschke S., Peters B., Rahm B., Kaiser J., & Bledowski C. (2020). Context information supports serial dependence of multiple visual objects across memory episodes. Nature Communications, 11(1), Article 1932. [16] Fischer, J., & Whitney, D. (2014). Serial dependence in visual perception. Nature Neuroscience, 17(5), 738-743. [17] Fornaciai, M., & Park, J. (2020). Attractive serial dependence between memorized stimuli. Cognition, 200, Article 104250. [18] Keetels M., Pecoraro M., & Vroomen J. (2015). Recalibration of auditory phonemes by lipread speech is ear-specific. Cognition, 141, 121-126. [19] Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003). Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 250-255. [20] Leys C., Ley C., Klein O., Bernard P., & Licata L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 764-766. [21] Li B. L., Wang B. Y., & Zaidel A. (2023). Modality-specific sensory and decisional carryover effects in duration perception. BMC Biology, 21(1), Article 48. [22] Li B. L., Yuan X. Y., & Huang X. T. (2015). The aftereffect of perceived duration is contingent on auditory frequency but not visual orientation. Scientific Reports, 5(1), Article 10124. [23] Manassi M., Ghirardo C., Canas-Bajo T., Ren Z. H., Prinzmetal W., & Whitney D. (2021). Serial dependence in the perceptual judgments of radiologists. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 6(1), Article 65. [24] Manassi M., Kristjánsson Á., & Whitney D. (2019). Serial dependence in a simulated clinical visual search task. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 19937. [25] Manassi, M., & Whitney, D. (2024). Continuity fields enhance visual perception through positive serial dependence. Nature Reviews Psychology, 3(5), 352-366. [26] Mochol G., Kiani R., & Moreno-Bote R. (2021). Prefrontal cortex represents heuristics that shape choice bias and its integration into future behavior. Current Biology, 31(6), 1234-1244.e6. [27] Moon, J., & Kwon, O. S. (2022). Attractive and repulsive effects of sensory history concurrently shape visual perception. BMC Biology, 20(1), Article 247. [28] Motala A., Zhang H. H., & Alais D. (2020). Auditory rate perception displays a positive serial dependence.I-Perception, 11(6), Article 2041669520982311. [29] Ortega L., Guzman-Martinez E., Grabowecky M., & Suzuki S. (2012). Flicker adaptation of low-level cortical visual neurons contributes to temporal dilation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1380-1389. [30] Pascucci D., Mancuso G., Santandrea E., Della Libera C., Plomp G., & Chelazzi L. (2019). Laws of concatenated perception: Vision goes for novelty, decisions for perseverance. PLoS Biology, 17(3), Article e3000144. [31] Togoli I., Fornaciai M., & Bueti D. (2022). Different modality-specific mechanisms mediate perceptual history effects in vision and audition. bioRxiv, Article 503081. [32] Wehrman J. J., Wearden J., & Sowman P. (2020). Decisional carryover effects in interval timing: Evidence of a generalized response bias. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 82(4), 2147-2164. [33] Wiener M., Thompson J. C., & Coslett H. B. (2014). Continuous carryover of temporal context dissociates response bias from perceptual influence for duration. PLoS ONE, 9(6), Article e100803. [34] Zhang, H. H., & Luo, H. (2023). Feature-specific reactivations of past information shift current neural encoding thereby mediating serial bias behaviors. PLoS Biology, 21(3), Article e3002056. [35] Zhou L. Q., Liu Y. J., Jiang Y. H., Wang W. B., Xu P. F., & Zhou K. (2024). The distinct development of stimulus and response serial dependence. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 31(5), 2137-2147. |