心理科学 ›› 2024, Vol. 47 ›› Issue (4): 829-837.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20240408
徐晓惠, 徐敏, 张耀华, 张明浩
出版日期:
2024-07-20
发布日期:
2024-07-17
通讯作者:
** 张明浩,E-mail: hellohao2004@163.com
基金资助:
Xu Xiaohui, Xu Min, Zhang Yaohua, Zhang Minghao
Online:
2024-07-20
Published:
2024-07-17
摘要: 程序公平是指决定分配的过程是否公平。已有研究使用资源分配范式,考察儿童的程序公平判断与程序公平行为的发展特点,发现程序公平判断在生命早期就已出现,幼儿能主动使用公平程序分配资源。然而,当公平程序与自身利益相冲突时,儿童仍然比较利己,出现程序公平的认知-行为差距。不偏袒公平理论和同等尊重理论可以解释儿童偏好程序公平的原因及儿童程序公平的发展特点。未来研究应侧重揭示儿童程序公平的发展轨迹并探讨其影响因素,增强儿童程序公平判断与程序公平行为发展同步性的实践探索,并验证不同理论观点的相对正确性。
徐晓惠, 徐敏, 张耀华, 张明浩. 儿童程序公平的发展特点及其内在机制*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(4): 829-837.
Xu Xiaohui, Xu Min, Zhang Yaohua, Zhang Minghao. Development of Children's Procedural Fairness and Its Internal Mechanisms[J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2024, 47(4): 829-837.
[1] 陈思静, 徐烨超. (2020). “仁者”还是“智者”:第三方惩罚对惩罚者声誉的影响. 心理学报, 52(12), 1436-1451. [2] 丁芳, 刘颜蓥, 张露. (2018). 小学儿童程序公平认知及其归因取向的发展. 心理科学, 41(2), 353-367. [3] 刘璐, 肖雪, 刘丽莎, 徐良苑, 张旭然, 李燕芳. (2019). 儿童在不同卷入情境下基于资源价值的分配行为特点:内群体偏爱的作用. 心理学报, 51(5), 584-597. [4] 刘文, 张雪, 张玉, 俞睿玮. (2017). 4~8岁儿童公平认知与行为差距:社会比较的作用. 心理学报, 49(12), 1504-1512. [5] 王笑楠, 郝洋, 苏彦捷. (2019). 争和合作对学龄前儿童分配模式的影响:心理理论和抑制控制的作用. 心理发展与教育, 35(4), 385-392. [6] 张雪, 刘文, 支焕. (2018). 6-8岁儿童表现公平行为的发展:社会信号的作用. 心理科学, 41(2), 324-329. [7] 张野, 张珊珊, 冯春莹. (2020). 不同程度的社会排斥情境对儿童程序公平感的影响. 贵州师范大学学报(自然科学版), 38(4), 94-100. [8] 张振, 齐春辉, 王洋, 赵辉, 王小新, 高晓雷. (2020). 内群体偏爱或黑羊效应?经济博弈中公平规范执行的群体偏见. 心理科学进展, 28(2), 329-339. [9] Acar, M., & Sivis, O. (2023). “But the poor needed it more”: Children' s judgments on procedural justice to allocate resources between two candidates equal in merit, different in need. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 232, 1-12. [10] Bird R. B., Ready E., & Power E. A. (2018). The social significance of subtle signals. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(7), 452-457. [11] Blake, P. R. (2018). Giving what one should: Explanations for the knowledge-behavior gap for altruistic giving. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 1-5. [12] Blake P. R., McAuliffe K., & Warneken F. (2014). The developmental origins of fairness: the knowledge-behavior gap. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(11), 559-561. [13] Blake P. R., Piovesan M., Montinari N., Warneken F., & Gino F. (2015). Prosocial norms in the classroom: The role of self-regulation in following norms of giving. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 115, 18-29. [14] Chai, Q., & He, J. (2017). Chinese preschoolers' resource allocation in the face of existing inequality under collaborative and noncollaborative contexts. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1494-1500. [15] Chernyak N., Leimgruber K. L., Dunham Y. C., Hu J., & Blake P. R. (2019). Paying back people who harmed us but not people who helped us: Direct negative reciprocity precedes direct positive reciprocity in early development. Psychological Science, 30(9), 1273-1286. [16] Corbit, J. (2019). Increased sharing between collaborators extends beyond the spoils of collaboration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 186, 159-170. [17] Dawkins M. B., Sloane S., & Baillargeon R. (2019). Do infants in the first year of life expect equal resource allocations? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1-19. [18] Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3), 137-149. [19] Dhaliwal N. A., Patil I., & Cushman F. (2021). Reputational and cooperative benefits of third-party compensation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 164, 27-51. [20] Douneva M., Jaffe M. E., & Greifeneder R. (2019). Toss and turn or toss and stop? A coin flip reduces the need for information in decision-making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 83, 132-141. [21] Dunham Y., Durkin A., & Tyler T. R. (2018). The development of a preference for procedural justice for self and others. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1-8. [22] Elenbaas, L. (2019). Against unfairness: Young children's judgments about merit, equity, and equality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 186, 73-82. [23] Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children's Sense of Fairness as Equal Respect. Trends in Cognitive Science, 23(6), 454-463. [24] Essler S., Christner N., Becher T., & Paulus M. (2023). The ontogenetic emergence of normativity: How action imitation relates to infants' norm enforcement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 227,1-15. [25] Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of voice and improvement on experienced inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 108-119. [26] Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management. SAGE Publications, Inc. [27] Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M. (2015). Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 197-210. [28] Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M. (2018). Young children are more willing to accept group decisions in which they have had a voice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 67-78. [29] Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M. (2019). Preschoolers consider (absent) others when choosing a distribution procedure. PloS ONE, 14(8), 1-14. [30] Heuer, L., & Stroessner, S. J. (2011). The multi-value basis of procedural justice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 541-553. [31] House B. R., Kanngiesser P., Barrett H. C., Broesch T., Cebioglu S., Crittenden A. N., & Silk J. B. (2020). Universal norm psychology leads to societal diversity in prosocial behaviour and development. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(1), 36-44. [32] Jacobs C., Flowers M., Aboody R., Maier M., & Jara-Ettinger J. (2022). Not just what you did, but how: Children see distributors that count as more fair than distributors who don't. Cognition, 225(5),1-11. [33] Keysar B., Converse B. A., Wang J., & Epley N. (2008). Reciprocity is not give and take:asymmetric reciprocity to positive and negative acts. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1280-1286. [34] Killen, M. (2018). The origins of morality: Social equality, fairness, and justice. Philosophical Psychology, 31(5), 767-803. [35] Knofe H., Engelmann J., Tomasello M., & Herrmann E. (2019). Chimpanzees monopolize and children take turns in a limited resource problem. Scientific Reports, 9(1),1-7. [36] Kuzmak, S. D., & Gelman, R. (1986). Young children's understanding of random phenomena. Child Devlopment, 57(3), 559-566. [37] Lenz S., Essler S., Worle M., & Paulus M. (2021). "Who will share with me?": Preschoolers rely on their friends more than on their nonfriends to share with them. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 203, 1-16. [38] Li W., Curtis R., Moore C., Wang Y., & Zeng X. (2019). To be fair, generous, or selfish: The effect of relationship on Chinese children' s distributive allocation and procedural application. Social Development, 28(2), 449-464. [39] Liberman, Z., & Shaw, A. (2017). Children use partial resource sharing as a cue to friendship. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 96-109. [40] Lind, E., & Tyler, T. (1988). The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice. Plenum press. [41] Manrique H. M., Zeidler H., Roberts G., Barclay P., Walker M., Samu, F. & Raihani, N. (2021). The psychological foundations of reputation-based cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1838),1-11. [42] Malti T., Peplak J., & Zhang L. L. (2020). The Development of Respect in Children and Adolescents. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 85(3), 7-98. [43] Margoni F., Baillargeon R., andSurian L. (2018). Infants distinguish between leaders and bullies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(38), 35-43. [44] Messer E. J. E., Lumsden A., Burgess V., & McGuigan N. (2021). Young children selectively adopt sharing norms according to norm content and donor age. Cognitive Development, 59,1-14. [45] Moreland, R. L. (2010). Are dyads really groups? Small Group Research, 41(2), 251-267. [46] Olivier J. L., McCall C., Dunham Y., & Over H. (2022). Procedural (in)justice in children: Children choose procedures that favor their ingroup. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 215, 1-21. [47] Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2017). Preschoolers' generosity increases with understanding of the affective benefits of sharing. Developmental Science, 20(3), 1-9. [48] Peretz-Lange R., Harvey T., & Blake P. R. (2022). Socioeconomic status predicts children' s moral judgments of novel resource distributions. Developmental Science, 25(4),1-7. [49] Qiu X., Yu J., Li T., Cheng N., & Zhu L. (2017). Children' s inequity aversion in procedural justice context: A comparison of advantageous and disadvantageous inequity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1-8. [50] Rapp D. J., Engelmann J. M., Herrmann E., & Tomasello M. (2019). Young children's reputational strategies in a peer group context. Developmental Psychology, 55(2), 329-336. [51] Samek A., Cowell J. M., Cappelen A. W., Cheng Y. W., Contreras-Ibáñez C., Gomez-Sicard, N. & Decety, J. (2020). The development of social comparisons and sharing behavior across 12 countries. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 192, 1-16. [52] Schafer M., Haun D. B. M., & Tomasello M. (2023). Children's consideration of collaboration and merit when making sharing decisions in private. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 228,1-18. [53] Serang S., Grimm K. J., & Zhang Z. (2018). On the correspondence between the latent growth curve and latent change score models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 26(4), 623-635. [54] Shaw, A. (2013). Beyond "to Share or Not to Share". Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 413-417. [55] Shaw A., Barakzai A., & Keysar B. (2019). When and why people evaluate negative reciprocity as more fair than positive reciprocity. Cognitive Science, 43(8), 1-35. [56] Shaw A., Choshen-Hillel S., & Caruso E. M. (2016). The development of inequity aversion: Understanding when (and why) people give others the bigger piece of the pie. Psychological Science, 27(10), 1352-1359. [57] Shaw A., Montinari N., Piovesan M., Olson K. R., Gino F., & Norton M. I. (2014). Children develop a veil of fairness. Journal of Experimental Psychological General, 143(1), 363-375. [58] Shaw, A., & Olson, K. (2014). Fairness as partiality aversion: the development of procedural justice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 40-53. [59] Sparks E., Schinkel M. G., & Moore C. (2017). Affiliation affects generosity in young children: The roles of minimal group membership and shared interests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 242-262. [60] Stowe L. M., Peretz-Lange R., & Blake P. R. (2022). Children consider procedures, outcomes, and emotions when judging the fairness of inequality. Frontiers in Psychology, 13,1-9. [61] Strauss, S., & Bondu, R. (2023). Fair sharing is just caring: Links between justice sensitivity and distributive behavior in middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 226,1-25. [62] Surian, L., & Margoni, F. (2020). First steps toward an understanding of procedural fairness. Developmental Science, 23(5), 1-12. [63] Tata, J. (2005). The influence of national culture on the perceived fairness of grading procedures: A comparison of the United States and China. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 139(5), 401-412. [64] Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual review of psychology, 64, 231-255. [65] Vogelsang, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Giving is nicer than taking: Preschoolers reciprocate based on the social intentions of the distributor. Plos ONE, 11(1), 1-12. [66] Wei B. Y., Zhang X. R., Xiao X., & Li Y. F. (2023). The effect of different types of social norms on children's sharing behavior: The roles of parents, teachers, and peers. Social Development, 32(3),1-19. [67] Xie D., Pei M., & Su Y. (2019). "Favoring my playmate seems fair": Inhibitory control and theory of mind in preschoolers' self-disadvantaging behaviors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 184, 158-173. [68] Yang Z., Zheng Y., Wang C. S., Lai X. Y., Hu K. S., Li Q., & Liu X. (2022). Fairness decision-making of opportunity equity in gain and loss contexts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 98,1-12. [69] Zhang X. R., Corbit J., Xiao X., Xu L. Y., Wei B. Y., & Li Y. F. (2021). Material and relational asymmetry: The role of receivers' wealth and power status in children's resource allocation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 208, 1-20. |
[1] | 刘旭, 刘宇潇, 陈倩, 曹敏, 彭霁, 周宗奎. 儿童友谊质量与主观幸福感和孤独感的双向关系:一项纵向研究*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(4): 819-828. |
[2] | 宋宜琪, 朱楚城, 梁丹丹. 汉语高功能自闭症儿童词汇语义加工中的空间象似效应——兼与图片知觉加工的比较*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(3): 554-561. |
[3] | 史滋福, 陈枭豪. 嵌套集合建模能力和数字表征对儿童贝叶斯推理的影响*[J]. 心理科学, 2024, 47(2): 274-280. |
[4] | 辛聪, 刘国雄, 程黎. 儿童前瞻记忆:执行功能的作用 *[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(6): 1360-1367. |
[5] | 吴岩, 陈启杨, 胡诗茜. 世界知识和词汇关联在10~12岁儿童句子加工中的作用:眼动研究 *[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(5): 1074-1080. |
[6] | 方浩宇, 祝孝亮, 赵鑫. 睡眠质量对10~12岁儿童学业成绩的影响:刷新能力的中介作用 *[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(5): 1090-1097. |
[7] | 李菲菲, 方海燕, 陈何, 刘宝根. 自闭症儿童的内隐学习假说:来自人工语法学习的证据*[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(4): 809-816. |
[8] | 孙倩 李晴蕾 刘永芳. 异性互动情境中程序公平对合作行为的影响:有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(2): 411-418. |
[9] | 陈永香 朱莉琪. 两岁儿童利用身体部位信息快速识别动词[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(2): 355-362. |
[10] | 魏玲 王静 余宥依 刘人豪 白学军. 自闭症谱系障碍儿童对面孔加工的同龄偏向效应[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(1): 72-81. |
[11] | 王振宏. 儿童发展的环境敏感性模型:一种整合的观点[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(6): 1367-1374. |
[12] | 范伟 傅文静 张文洁 刘繁 钟毅平. 听障儿童错误信念理解与谎言理解的关系:情绪理解的调节作用[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(5): 1123-1135. |
[13] | 靳羽西 梁丹丹. 高功能自闭症儿童不同常规度转喻加工的发展:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(4): 871-878. |
[14] | 王英杰 张美霞 谢庆斌 李燕. 母亲养育压力与学前儿童社会适应的关系及其性别差异:一项交叉滞后分析[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(3): 620-627. |
[15] | Yananda Nana Madhlopa 陈晨 杜雨航 丁崇淑 秦金亮. 儿童虐待与问题行为:基因与环境的作用[J]. 心理科学, 2022, 45(3): 650-656. |
阅读次数 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
全文 862
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
摘要 490
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||