心理科学 ›› 2024, Vol. 47 ›› Issue (4): 829-837.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20240408

• 发展与教育 • 上一篇    下一篇

儿童程序公平的发展特点及其内在机制*

徐晓惠, 徐敏, 张耀华, 张明浩   

  1. 鲁东大学教育科学学院,新旧动能转换与黄河流域青少年心理健康协同创新中心,烟台,264025
  • 出版日期:2024-07-20 发布日期:2024-07-17
  • 通讯作者: ** 张明浩,E-mail: hellohao2004@163.com
  • 基金资助:
    *本研究得到教育部人文社会科学研究青年基金项目(23YJC880119)的资助

Development of Children's Procedural Fairness and Its Internal Mechanisms

Xu Xiaohui, Xu Min, Zhang Yaohua, Zhang Minghao   

  1. School of Educational Science, Collaborative Innovation Center for the Mental Health of Youth from the Era of Conversion of New and Old Kinetic Energy along the Yellow River Basin, Ludong University, Yantai, 264025
  • Online:2024-07-20 Published:2024-07-17

摘要: 程序公平是指决定分配的过程是否公平。已有研究使用资源分配范式,考察儿童的程序公平判断与程序公平行为的发展特点,发现程序公平判断在生命早期就已出现,幼儿能主动使用公平程序分配资源。然而,当公平程序与自身利益相冲突时,儿童仍然比较利己,出现程序公平的认知-行为差距。不偏袒公平理论和同等尊重理论可以解释儿童偏好程序公平的原因及儿童程序公平的发展特点。未来研究应侧重揭示儿童程序公平的发展轨迹并探讨其影响因素,增强儿童程序公平判断与程序公平行为发展同步性的实践探索,并验证不同理论观点的相对正确性。

关键词: 程序公平, 分配公平, 不偏袒公平理论, 同等尊重理论, 儿童

Abstract: In the past decades, researchers payed primary attention to distributive fairness, and they found that children prefer distributive fairness. However, it is unknown that whether children also prefer procedural fairness. Procedural fairness refers to whether the methods, mechanisms, and processes that determine the outcome are fair. Researchers often ask children to use "procedures" to allocate resources, such as spinning a wheel, tossing a coin, rolling a die, rotating an order, etc. These fair procedures share two important characteristics: randomness of outcome and equality of opportunity. Research has found that infants already possess sensitivity of procedural fairness, and children aged 3 to 5 were able to accurately judge whether an allocation procedure is fair and would protest against unfair procedures. As they grow, children demonstrate more mature procedural fairness behavior. However, children are not always able to follow procedural fairness norms to guide their distributive behavior, which refers to cognition-behavior gap. That is, the development of children's procedural behavior lags behind their perception of procedural fairness. For example, it has been found that children aged 4 to 6 prefer unfair procedures that are favorable to themselves. It is not until around 8 years old that children can reject unfair procedures in their favor.
Impartiality Account of Fairness and Fairness as Equal Respect have usually been used to explain the development characteristics of children's procedural fairness. The former theory suggests that fairness functions to signal the fair individual's impartiality to others, aimed to build a good reputation for the divider. It has been found that, as children grow, they become increasingly concerned whether they are impartial and try to build a reputation for pro-sociality within their peer group. For example, children aged 7 to 9 are more likely to allocate resources by flipping a coin than those aged 4 to 6 because they are worried about their reputation being damaged. Impartiality has two forms: first, when children were distributors, they would create inequality that disadvantaged to themselves rather than to peers. Second, children would fairly treat all the members in a distribution in order to avoid partiality to someone. The latter theory emphasizes that children's sense of fairness is rooted in their need for equal respect. The “respect” can be interpreted in two aspects. One means that children have an opportunity to voice on their behalf, and the other means that children are not just concerned about the material rewards, they are concerned about the social meaning of the distributive action. It has been found that as children grow older, they become increasingly sensitive to respect, especially when they are treated unfairly. Compared to Impartiality Account of Fairness, Fairness as Equal Respect is more inclusive. In procedural fairness, equal respect is the core, while impartiality is the extrinsic manifestation.
Future research can start from three aspects. First, to reveal the developmental trajectory of children's procedural fairness and elucidate the dynamic interactive developmental process between children's procedural fairness perceptions and interfering factors, such as selfish motives, social comparison, group identity and social economic status etc. Second, to enhance the theoretical and practical explorations of the synchronicity between the development of children's procedural fairness perceptions and procedural fairness behavior, and to further explore the role of collaborative activities in bridging the cognition-behavior gap. Last but not least, future studies could design triadic interaction contexts to examine the decision-making patterns of the intermediate-positioned individuals. For example, whether they punish the one who gained more and compensate the one who gained less or do nothing. This may help to validate the relative correctness of the theory of Impartiality Account of Fairness and Fairness as Equal Respect.

Key words: procedural fairness, distributive fairness, impartiality account of fairness, fairness as equal respect, children