Journal of Psychological Science ›› 2023, Vol. 46 ›› Issue (6): 1360-1367.DOI: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20230611
• Developmental & Educational Psychology • Previous Articles Next Articles
Xin Cong1, Liu Guoxiong1, Cheng Li2,3
Published:
2023-12-19
辛聪1, 刘国雄1, 程黎**2,3
通讯作者:
**程黎,E-mail: licheng1lcl@126.com
基金资助:
Xin Cong, Liu Guoxiong, Cheng Li. Children’s Prospective Memory:The Role of Executive Functions[J]. Journal of Psychological Science, 2023, 46(6): 1360-1367.
辛聪, 刘国雄, 程黎. 儿童前瞻记忆:执行功能的作用 *[J]. 心理科学, 2023, 46(6): 1360-1367.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://jps.ecnu.edu.cn/EN/10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20230611
[1] 周晨琛, 姬鸣, 兰楠, 黄欢, 游旭群. (2017). 基于事件的前瞻记忆与认知控制. 心理科学, 40(4), 856-862. [2] Altgassen M., Vetter N. C., Phillips L. H., Akgün C., & Kliegel M. (2014). Theory of mind and switching predict prospective memory performance in adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 127, 163-175. [3] Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660. [4] Causey, K. B., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2014). Prospective memory in preschool children: Influences of agency, incentive, and underlying cognitive mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 127, 36-51. [5] Cheie L., MacLeod C., Miclea M., & Visu-Petra L. (2017). When children forget to remember: Effects of reduced working memory availability on prospective memory performance. Memory and Cognition, 45(4), 651-663. [6] Chevalier N., Jackson J., Roux A. R., Moriguchi Y., & Auyeung B. (2019). Differentiation in prefrontal cortex recruitment during childhood: Evidence from cognitive control demands and social contexts. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 36, Article 100629. [7] Cohen A. L., Gordon A., Jaudas A., Hefer C., & Dreisbach G. (2017). Let it go: The flexible engagement and disengagement of monitoring processes in a non-focal prospective memory task. Psychological Research, 81(2), 366-377. [8] Cona G., Scarpazza C., Sartori G., Moscovitch M., & Bisiacchi P. S. (2015). Neural bases of prospective memory: A meta-analysis and the “Attention to Delayed Intention” (AtoDI) model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 52, 21-37. [9] Cuevas, K., & Bell, M. A. (2014). Infant attention and early childhood executive function. Child Development, 85(2), 397-404. [10] Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135-168. [11] Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 717-726. [12] Einstein G. O., McDaniel M. A., Thomas R., Mayfield S., Shank H., Morrisette N., & Breneiser J. (2005). Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: Factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 327-342. [13] Ezekiel F., Bosma R., & Morton J. B. (2013). Dimensional change card sort performance associated with age-related differences in functional connectivity of lateral prefrontal cortex. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 40-50. [14] Fiske, A., & Holmboe, K. (2019). Neural substrates of early executive function development. Developmental Review, 52, 42-62. [15] Ford R. M., Driscoll T., Shum D., & Macaulay C. E. (2012). Executive and theory-of-mind contributions to event-based prospective memory in children: Exploring the self-projection hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 468-489. [16] Kliegel M., Brandenberger M., & Aberle I. (2010). Effect of motivational incentives on prospective memory performance in preschoolers. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(2), 223-232. [17] Kliegel, M., & Jäger, T. (2007). The effects of age and cue-action reminders on event-based prospective memory performance in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 22(1), 33-46. [18] Kliegel M., Mackinlay R., & Jäger T. (2008). Complex prospective memory: Development across the lifespan and the role of task interruption. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 612-617. [19] Kretschmer A., Voigt B., Friedrich S., Pfeiffer K., & Kliegel M. (2014). Time-based prospective memory in young children—Exploring executive functions as a developmental mechanism. Child Neuropsychology, 20(6), 662-676. [20] Lee K., Bull R., & Ho, R. M. H. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development, 84(6), 1933-1953. [21] Mahy, C. E. V., & Moses, L. J. (2011). Executive functioning and prospective memory in young children. Cognitive Development, 26(3), 269-281. [22] Mahy C. E. V., Moses L. J., & Kliegel M. (2014a). The development of prospective memory in children: An executive framework. Developmental Review, 34(4), 305-326. [23] Mahy C. E. V., Moses L. J., & Kliegel M. (2014b). The impact of age, ongoing task difficulty, and cue salience on preschoolers'prospective memory performance: The role of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 127, 52-64. [24] Mahy, C. E. V., & Munakata, Y. (2015). Transitions in executive function: Insights from developmental parallels between prospective memory and cognitive flexibility. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 128-132. [25] Mäntylä T., Carelli M. G., & Forman H. (2007). Time monitoring and executive functioning in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(1), 1-19. [26] McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), S127-S144. [27] McDaniel M. A., LaMontagne P., Beck S. M., Scullin M. K., & Braver T. S. (2013). Dissociable neural routes to successful prospective memory. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1791-1800. [28] Meier, B., & Zimmermann, T. D. (2015). Loads and loads and loads: The influence of prospective load, retrospective load, and ongoing task load in prospective memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, Article 322. [29] Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14. [30] Perlman S. B., Huppert T. J., & Luna B. (2016). Functional near-infrared spectroscopy evidence for development of prefrontal engagement in working memory in early through middle childhood. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6), 2790-2799. [31] Redshaw J., Vandersee J., Bulley A., & Gilbert S. J. (2018). Development of children’s use of external reminders for hard-to-remember intentions. Child Development, 89(6), 2099-2108. [32] Rubia K., Smith A. B., Taylor E., & Brammer M. (2007). Linear age-correlated functional development of right inferior fronto-striato-cerebellar networks during response inhibition and anterior cingulate during error-related processes. Human Brain Mapping, 28(11), 1163-1177. [33] Sheppard D. P., Matchanova A., Sullivan K. L., Kazimi S. I., & Woods S. P. (2020). Prospective memory partially mediates the association between aging and everyday functioning. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(4), 755-774. [34] Shum D., Cross B., Ford R., & Ownsworth T. (2008). A developmental investigation of prospective memory: Effects of interruption. Child Neuropsychology, 14(6), 547-561. [35] Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 347-361. [36] Somerville S. C., Wellman H. M., & Cultice J. C. (1983). Young children's deliberate reminding. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 143(1), 87-96. [37] Spiess M. A., Meier B., & Roebers C. M. (2016). Development and longitudinal relationships between children's executive functions, prospective memory, and metacognition. Cognitive Development, 38, 99-113. [38] Tamnes C. K., Østby Y., Walhovd K. B., Westlye L. T., Due-Tønnessen P., & Fjell A. M. (2010). Neuroanatomical correlates of executive functions in children and adolescents: A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study of cortical thickness. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2496-2508. [39] Thompson, A., & Steinbeis, N. (2020). Sensitive periods in executive function development. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 98-105. [40] Voigt B., Mahy C. E. V., Ellis J., Schnitzspahn K., Krause I., Altgassen M., & Kliegel M. (2014). The development of time-based prospective memory in childhood: The role of working memory updating. Developmental Psychology, 50(10), 2393-2404. [41] Williams D., Boucher J., Lind S., & Jarrold C. (2013). Time-based and event-based prospective memory in autism spectrum disorder: The roles of executive function and theory of mind, and time-estimation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 1555-1567. [42] Yang T. X., Chan R. C. K., & Shum D. (2011). The development of prospective memory in typically developing children. Neuropsychology, 25(3), 342-352. [43] Zimmermann, T. D., & Meier, B. (2006). The rise and decline of prospective memory performance across the lifespan. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(12), 2040-2046. [44] Zuber, S., & Kliegel, M. (2020). Prospective memory development across the lifespan: An integrative framework. European Psychologist, 25(3), 162-173. [45] Zuber S., Mahy C. E. V., & Kliegel M. (2019). How executive functions are associated with event-based and time-based prospective memory during childhood. Cognitive Development, 50, 66-79. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||